Developmental Essay.docx

  • Uploaded by: Ellen Frances
  • 0
  • 0
  • October 2019
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Developmental Essay.docx as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 2,878
  • Pages: 11
HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT? CANDIDATE NUMBER: 148917

The following essay looks at the issue of child temperament and the Chess and Thomas (1984) theory of child temperament that categorizes children according to their behavioural styles (as cited in Shiner et al., 2012). The essay will then look at the subsequent issues that arise as a result of childhood temperament styles. For example, issues with personality development, emotional adjustment, the development of aggressive behavioural problems, as well as the effect temperament has on a child’s attachment type and therefore their ability to form stable relationships in the future. Temperament is often defined as the way in which a child behaves (Thomas & Chess, 1977). One of the most commonly used theories of temperament is the Chess and Thomas (1984) approach (as cited in Shiner et al., 2012). This approach looks specifically at the variations in children’s behaviour from early infancy and labels the patterns in behaviour as different types temperament. Chess and Thomas (1984) stated that all children fall into three temperament types (easy, difficult, and slow to warm up) based on the behaviour observed by researchers.

Following the development of this theory, psychologists began to look at the effect of childhood temperament on the different aspects of development. Research has shown that the level of temperament in infancy influences different aspects of the development of personality (Caspi et al., 2003). Personality is an extremely well researched topic in psychology, and one of the most established theories is that of Goldberg (1990), who suggested that there are 5 different components of personality: extraversion, agreeableness, openness, neuroticism, and conscientiousness. The way in which these components of personality develop often influences

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

2

a person’s risk of developing various issues. For example, research has shown that neuroticism and openness often predict whether a person is likely to develop depression (Chioqueta & Stiles, 2005), and extraversion often predicts global anxiety (Gershuny & Sher, 1998). Also, it should be noted, that there is a high prevalence of personality disorders these days (Torgersen, Kringlen, & Cramer, 2001), and therefore, psychologists have looked to find the factors that predict the development of positive and negative personality traits to help prevent the development of depression, anxiety, BPD, and other similar disorders.

It was from this, that researchers began looking at the different aspects of child development that may or may not influence the type of person you become in the future. Because temperament is a childhood trait, and because childhood is a key developmental period particularly for personality (Caspi, Roberts, & Shiner, 2005), psychologists assumed an association between childhood temperament and personality development, which resulted in a wealth of research looking to find types of childhood temperament that cause the development of negative personality traits. Caspi and Silva (1995) studied this by looking at how a child’s behavioural style is linked to their personality traits as adults. Following observations of children at age 3, examiners identified 5 different behavioural styles, including uncontrolled, inhibited, confident, reserved, and well-adjusted. Following a personality questionnaire administered at age 18, the study found that uncontrolled and confident children were more impulsive as adults, whereas inhibited children were much less impulsive as adults. However, this study did not account for individual differences in child development. A child may be uncontrolled and confident to begin with, but research has shown that individual life events can affect the way a person’s personality develops (Block, Block, & Gierde, 1986) and therefore not every uncontrolled and confident child will grow up to be impulsive. Nevertheless, the findings of Caspi and Silva (1995) are significant and therefore it was

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

3

concluded that children who were more difficult in temperament were subsequently more impulsive as adults.

From these findings, psychologists began to see the link between childhood temperament and future behaviour. Therefore, researchers have become increasingly interested in whether temperament is linked to specific behavioural problems in adults. As a result of the research of Caspi and Silva (1995), it was assumed that children who were more difficult in temperament may be more aggressive and/or violent as a result an inability to inhibit their behaviours and therefore act impulsively. In response to this, research began looking at specific behavioural problems in order to form the basis for preventative techniques. Henry, Caspi, Moffitt and Silva (1996) looked specifically at the link between childhood temperament at age 3 and future violent and non-violent convictions at age 18. They found that that childhood temperament (i.e. lack of control) was a strong determinant of whether a person was convicted of violent or non-violent crime in the future. This suggests that children who are more difficult in temperament are more likely to commit a violent crime. Similarly, Veenstra, Lindenberg, Oldehinkel, De Winter and Ormel (2006) found that temperament, in terms of the capacity to voluntarily regulate behaviour and attention, was significantly associated with antisocial behaviour, a behaviour that is closely linked to the likelihood of a person committing violent and/or non-violent crime in the future (Loeber, 1982). These findings provide a strong basis for the development of preventative techniques, to reduce the rate of crime. For example, intervention at age 3, to decrease the difficulty of a child’s temperament, may subsequently reduce antisocial behaviour and therefore reduce the rate of violent and non-violent crime.

Similarly, interventions that target children’s temperament may also reduce the rate of substance abuse in adults. This is because it has been found that childhood temperament

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

4

influences a person’s risk of drug and alcohol use, as well as substance abuse in general. Gerra et al. (2004) did a study on whether a person’s score on the Zuckerman Sensation seeking scale, the Eysenck personality questionnaire, the Buss-Durkee hostility inventory, and the parental bonding instrument, predicted that person’s risk of life time alcohol and drug usage. The study found that many different aspects of a person’s personality were associated with drug use and alcohol abuse, and that these personality differences may be a result of temperament (Caspi & Silva, 1995). However, the findings also suggested that temperament and personality are often associated with the perception of poor parental care and low ratings of parental care often resulted in early substance use. Therefore, it was concluded that there is a link between temperament, perception of parental care, and a person’s risk of substance abuse. However, in this case intervention at the age of childhood temperament (i.e. age 3) would not be appropriate. This is because the research shows that it is not just temperament that increases the risk of substance abuse. Therefore, interventions would have to incorporate techniques that target a person’s need to sensation seek, aggression and low parental care as well as temperament, which is much more invasive as it targets aspects of personality rather than just the child’s behaviour.

Another predictor of substance abuse and crime is conduct disorder (Copur, Turkcan, & Erdogmus, 2005), and research has shown that a person’s risk of conduct disorder may be predicted by childhood temperament. Caspi, Henry, McGee, Moffitt and Silva (1995) sampled 800 children to see if temperament at age 3 effected behaviour at age 15. They found that Lack of control at age 3 and 5 increased future externalizing problems such as conduct disorder. This suggests that childhood temperament is a strong predictor of conduct problems in the future. However, much like the study Caspi and Silva (1995) this study does not account for individual differences, particularly in psychological resilience. Therefore, even someone who is at risk of

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

5

developing conduct disorder may never develop it. However, other research has found similar findings to Caspi et al., (1995). For example, Lahey et al. (2008) found that parentally rated “difficult” temperament (i.e. fussiness, activity level, and predictability) during an infant’s first year was predicted by early parenting style, and that this “difficult” temperament was a predictor of conduct problems, suggesting that there is definitely a link between temperament and conduct disorder. Therefore, supporting the link between temperament and substance abuse/criminal activity.

Caspi et al. (2003) did another study that examined the same behavioural styles of children at age 3 as Caspi and Silva (1995), and then looked at the subsequent personality traits that resulted at age 18 and age 26. They found similar results in that children who appeared uncontrolled at age 3 were more impulsive at age 18, but also at age 26 showing that these personality traits are consistent over time. It was also found that uncontrolled children were more emotionally sensitive at age 26 than children who were more inhibited. Because emotional sensitivity is often associated with disorders of emotion such as borderline personality disorder (BPD; Lynch et al., 2006), and because behavioural features of BPD are similar to those of depression (Luca, Luca, & Calandra, 2012), it may be assumed that because children who were more difficult in temperament are more emotionally sensitive (Caspi et al., 2003) they may be more susceptible to future emotional problems such as depression (Clark, Watson, & Mineka, 1994). Other theories suggest that temperament accounts for the variation in the rate of depression between males and females (Cloninger, Syrakic, & Pryzbeck, 2006), and acts as a moderator on stress responses subsequently effecting the ability to cope with depression (Compas, Conner-Smith, & Jaser, 2004). Either way, it can be concluded that temperament is a strong predictor of differences in personality traits, and because of this,

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

6

predicts differences in the rate behavioural problems and a person’s susceptibility to emotional or personality disorders such as depression, anxiety, and BPD.

Alternatively, other researchers have looked at the link between childhood temperament and subsequent attachment to a primary caregiver. Ainsworth and Bell (1970) categorized infants in terms of the style of their attachment to a maternal figure. The strange situation experiment identified three main attachment types, a secure attachment, and insecure avoidant attachment, and an insecure ambivalent/resistant. There are many different things that can affect a child’s attachment type. Research has suggested that significant predictors of attachment types are both maternal sensitivity (Smith & Pederson, 1988) and child temperament (Vaughn et al., 1992). Boom (1994) found that the children whose mothers had received interventions in order to enhance their sensitivity and responsiveness in general had a more secure attachment and increase exploration, suggesting that this is a significant predictor of attachment type. Alternatively, Seifer et al. (1996) found that temperament moderately predicted security of attachment, and that maternal sensitivity no longer accounted for attachment when temperament was being accounted for; suggesting a strong link between childhood temperament and attachment independent of maternal sensitivity. Research suggests that a child’s attachment type effects certain aspects of their development and therefore influences the sort of person they are in the future. For example, Hazan and Shaver (1987) found that the way people experience romantic relationships as adults is similar to the attachment type they have as a child. So much so that people who had secure romantic relationships, were found to of had a secure attachment as a child, and people who rarely fall in love, often had an insecure avoidant childhood attachment. From this we can conclude that childhood temperament may have an indirect influence on how a person approaches future relationships, because of the style of attachment a person once had to their primary caregiver.

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

7

In conclusion, childhood temperament is categorized as either easy, difficult, or slow to warm up. Research has suggested that a child’s temperament type effects the development of their personality traits and subsequently increases their risk of the development of poor emotional adjustment resulting in disorders such as depression, anxiety, and BPD. There is also evidence to suggest that temperament influences how aggressive a person is in the future, and whether a person is at increased risk of substance abuse and/or crime, either directly or indirectly due to an increased risk of conduct problems. It has also been found that temperament effects a child’s attachment to their primary caregiver and this subsequently effects the way they approach future relationships. These findings all provide a solid basis for the development of interventions based on preventative techniques. Further research can attempt to look at preventative techniques and see how much they reduce difficult temperament and then how much they reduce the subsequent issues that result from temperament styles.

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

8

References Ainsworth, M. D. S., & Bell, S. M. (1970). Attachment, exploration, and separation: Illustrated by the behavior of one-year-olds in a strange situation. Child development, 41(1), 4967. Block, J. H., Block, J., & Gjerde, P. F. (1986). The personality of children prior to divorce: A prospective study. Child development, 57, 827-840. Boom, D. C. (1994). The influence of temperament and mothering on attachment and exploration: An experimental manipulation of sensitive responsiveness among lower‐ class mothers with irritable infants. Child development, 65(5), 1457-1477. Caspi, A., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental qualities at age three predict personality traits in young adulthood: Longitudinal evidence from a birth cohort. Child development, 66(2), 486-498. Caspi, A., Harrington, H., Milne, B., Amell, J. W., Theodore, R. F., & Moffitt, T. E. (2003). Children's behavioral styles at age 3 are linked to their adult personality traits at age 26. Journal of personality, 71(4), 495-514. Caspi, A., Henry, B., McGee, R. O., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1995). Temperamental origins of child and adolescent behavior problems: From age three to age fifteen. Child development, 66(1), 55-68. Caspi, A., Roberts, B. W., & Shiner, R. L. (2005). Personality development: Stability and change. Annual Review of Psychology, 56, 453-484. Chioqueta, A. P., & Stiles, T. C. (2005). Personality traits and the development of depression, hopelessness, and suicide ideation. Personality and individual differences, 38(6), 12831291.

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

9

Compas, B. E., Connor-Smith, J., & Jaser, S. S. (2004). Temperament, stress reactivity, and coping: Implications for depression in childhood and adolescence. Journal of Clinical Child and Adolescent Psychology, 33(1), 21-31. Copur, M., Turkcan, A., & Erdogmus, M. (2005). Substance abuse, conduct disorder and crime: Assessment in a juvenile detention house in Istanbul, Turkey. Psychiatry and clinical neurosciences, 59(2), 151-154. Gerra, G., Angioni, L., Zaimovic, A., Moi, G., Bussandri, M., Bertacca, S., ... & Nicoli, M. A. (2004). Substance use among high-school students: relationships with temperament, personality traits, and parental care perception. Substance Use & Misuse, 39(2), 345367. Goldberg, L. R. (1990). An alternative" description of personality": the big-five factor structure. Journal of personality and social psychology, 59(6), 1216-1229. Hazan, C., & Shaver, P. (1987). Romantic love conceptualized as an attachment process. Journal of personality and social psychology, 52(3), 511-524. Henry, B., Caspi, A., Moffitt, T. E., & Silva, P. A. (1996). Temperamental and familial predictors of violent and nonviolent criminal convictions: Age 3 to age 18. Developmental psychology, 32(4), 614-623. Lahey, B. B., Van Hulle, C. A., Keenan, K., Rathouz, P. J., D’Onofrio, B. M., Rodgers, J. L., & Waldman, I. D. (2008). Temperament and parenting during the first year of life predict future child conduct problems. Journal of abnormal child psychology, 36(8), 1139-1158. Loeber, R. (1982). The stability of antisocial and delinquent child behavior: A review. Child development, 53(6), 1431-1446. Luca, M., Luca, A., & Calandra, C. (2012). Borderline personality disorder and depression: an update. Psychiatric Quarterly, 83(3), 281-292.

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

10

Lynch, T. R., Rosenthal, M. Z., Kosson, D. S., Cheavens, J. S., Lejuez, C. W., & Blair, R. J. R. (2006). Heightened sensitivity to facial expressions of emotion in borderline personality disorder. Emotion, 6(4), 647-655. McCrae, R. R., Costa Jr, P. T., Ostendorf, F., Angleitner, A., Hřebíčková, M., Avia, M. D., ... & Saunders, P. R. (2000). Nature over nurture: temperament, personality, and life span development. Journal of personality and social psychology, 78(1), 173-186. Seifer, R., Schiller, M., Sameroff, A. J., Resnick, S., & Riordan, K. (1996). Attachment, maternal

sensitivity,

and

infant

temperament

during

the

first

year

of

life. Developmental Psychology, 32(1), 12-25 Shiner, R. L., Buss, K. A., McClowry, S. G., Putnam, S. P., Saudino, K. J., & Zentner, M. (2012). What Is Temperament Now? Assessing Progress in Temperament Research on the Twenty‐Fifth Anniversary of Goldsmith et al. (1987). Child Development Perspectives, 6(4), 436-444. Smith, P. B., & Pederson, D. R. (1988). Maternal sensitivity and patterns of infant-mother attachment. Child development, 59(4), 1097-1101. Thomas, A., & Chess, S. (1977). Temperament and development. Oxford, England: Brunner/Mazel. Torgersen, S., Kringlen, E., & Cramer, V. (2001). The prevalence of personality disorders in a community sample. Archives of general psychiatry, 58(6), 590-596. Vaughn, B. E., Stevenson-Hinde, J., Waters, E., Kotsaftis, A., Lefever, G. B., Shouldice, A., ... & Belsky, J. (1992). Attachment security and temperament in infancy and early childhood: Some conceptual clarifications. Developmental Psychology, 28(3), 463473. Veenstra, R., Lindenberg, S., Oldehinkel, A. J., De Winter, A. F., & Ormel, J. (2006). Temperament, environment, and antisocial behavior in a population sample of

HOW DOES CHILD TEMPERAMENT RELATE TO SUBSEQUENT ADJUSTMENT?

11

preadolescent boys and girls. International Journal of Behavioral Development, 30(5), 422-432.

Related Documents

Developmental
July 2020 27
Developmental State
May 2020 7
Developmental Assessment
November 2019 28
Developmental Essay.docx
October 2019 29

More Documents from ""

Developmental Essay.docx
October 2019 29
Dapus.doc
April 2020 16
November 2019 18
August 2019 30