Why The Null Hypothesis Is Not Accepted

  • Uploaded by: Izi
  • 0
  • 0
  • May 2020
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Why The Null Hypothesis Is Not Accepted as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 1,578
  • Pages: 6
Why the Null Hypothesis is Not Accepted A null hypothesis is not accepted just because it is not rejected. Data not sufficient to show convincingly that a difference between means is not zero do not prove that the difference is zero. Such data may even suggest that the null hypothesis is false but not be strong enough to make a convincing case that the null hypothesis is false. For example, if the probability value were 0.15, then one would not be ready to present one's case that the null hypothesis is false to the (properly) skeptical scientific community. More convincing data would be needed to do that. However, there would be no basis to conclude that the null hypothesis is true. It may or may not be true, there just is not strong enough evidence to reject it. Not even in cases where there is no evidence that the null hypothesis is false is it valid to conclude the null hypothesis is true. If the null hypothesis is that µ1 - µ2 is zero then the hypothesis is that the difference is exactly zero. No experiment can distinguish between the case of no difference between means and an extremely small difference between means. If data are consistent with the null hypothesis, they are also consistent with other similar hypotheses. Thus, if the data do not provide a basis for rejecting the null hypothesis that µ1- µ2 = 0 then they almost certainly will not provide a basis for rejecting the hypothesis that µ1- µ2 = 0.001. The data are consistent with both hypotheses. When the null hypothesis is not rejected then it is legitimate to conclude that the data are consistent with the null hypothesis. It is not legitimate to conclude that the data support the acceptance of the null hypothesis since the data are consistent with other hypotheses as well. In some respects, rejecting the null hypothesis is comparable to a jury finding a defendant guilty. In both cases, the evidence is convincing beyond a reasonable doubt. Failing to reject the null hypothesis is comparable to a finding of not guilty. The defendant is not declared innocent. There is just not enough evidence to be convincing beyond a reasonable doubt. In the judicial system, a decision has to be made and the defendant is set free. In science, no decision has to be made immediately. More experiments are conducted.

One experiment might provide data sufficient to reject the null hypothesis, although no experiment can demonstrate that the null hypothesis is true. Where does this leave the researcher who wishes to argue that a variable does not have an effect? If the null hypothesis cannot be accepted, even in principle, then what type of statistical evidence can be used to support the hypothesis that a variable does not have an effect. The answer lies in relaxing the claim a little and arguing not that a variable has no effect whatsoever but that it has, at most, a negligible effect. This can be done by constructing a confidence interval around the parameter value. Consider a researcher interested in the possible effectiveness of a new psychotherapeutic drug. The researcher conducted an experiment comparing a drug-treatment group to a control group and found no significant difference between them. Although the experimenter cannot claim the drug has no effect, he or she can estimate the size of the effect using a confidence interval. If µ1 were the population mean for the drug group and µ2 were the population mean for the control group, then the confidence interval would be on the parameter µ1 - µ2. Assume the experiment measured "well being" on a 50 point scale (with higher scores representing more well being) that has a standard deviation of 10. Further assume the 99% confidence interval computed from the experimental data was: -0.5 ≤ µ1- µ2 ≤ 1 This says that one can be confident that the mean "true" drug treatment effect is somewhere between -0.5 and 1. If it were -0.5 then the drug would, on average, be slightly detrimental; if it were 1 then the drug would, on average, be slightly beneficial. But, how much benefit is an average improvement of 1? Naturally that is a question that involves characteristics of the measurement scale. But, since 1 is only 0.10 standard deviations, it can be presumed to

be a small effect. The overlap between two distributions whose means differ by 0.10 standard deviations is shown below. Although the blue distribution is

slightly to the right of the red distribution, the overlap is almost complete.

So, the finding that the maximum difference that can be expected (based on a 99% confidence interval) is itself a very small difference would allow the experimenter to conclude that the drug is not effective. The claim would not be that it is totally ineffective, but, at most, its effectiveness is very limited.

Next section: The precise meaning of the p value Confidence Intervals & Hypothesis Testing There is an extremely close relationship between confidence intervals and hypothesis testing. When a 95% confidence interval is constructed, all values in the interval are considered plausible values for the parameter being estimated. Values outside the interval are rejected as relatively implausible. If the value of the parameter specified by the null hypothesis is contained in the 95% interval then the null hypothesis cannot be rejected at the 0.05 level. If the value specified by the null hypothesis is not in the interval then the null hypothesis can be rejected at the 0.05 level. If a 99% confidence interval is constructed, then values outside the interval are rejected at the 0.01 level.

Imagine a researcher wishing to test the null hypothesis that the mean time to respond to an auditory signal is the same as the mean time to respond to a visual signal. The null hypothesis therefore is: μvisual - μauditory = 0. Ten subjects were tested in the visual condition and their scores (in milliseconds) were: 355, 421, 299, 460, 600, 580, 474, 511, 550, and 586. Ten subjects were tested in the auditory condition and their scores were: 275, 320, 278, 360, 430, 520, 464, 311, 529, and 326. The 95% confidence interval on the difference between means is: 9 ≤ μvisual - μauditory ≤ 196. Therefore only values in the interval between 9 and 196 are retained as plausible values for the difference between population means. Since zero, the value specified by the null hypothesis, is not in the interval, the null hypothesis of no difference between auditory and visual presentation can be rejected at the 0.05 level. The probability value for this example is 0.034. Any time the parameter specified by a null hypothesis is not contained in the 95% confidence interval estimating that parameter, the null hypothesis can be rejected at the 0.05 level or less. Similarly, if the 99% interval does not contain the parameter then the null hypothesis can be rejected at the 0.01 level. The null hypothesis is not rejected if the parameter value specified by the null hypothesis is in the interval since the null hypothesis would still be plausible. However, since the null hypothesis would be only one of an infinite number of values in the confidence interval, accepting the null hypothesis is not justified. There are many arguments against accepting the null hypothesis when it is not rejected. The null hypothesis is usually a hypothesis of

no difference. Thus null hypotheses such as: μ1 - μ 2 = 0 π 1 - π2 = 0 in which the hypothesized value is zero are most common. When the hypothesized value is zero then there is a simple relationship between hypothesis testing and confidence intervals: If the interval contains zero then the null hypothesis cannot be rejected at the stated level of confidence. If the interval does not contain zero then the null hypothesis can be rejected. This is just a special case of the general rule stating that the null hypothesis can be rejected if the interval does not contain the hypothesized value of the parameter and cannot be rejected if the interval contains the hypothesized value. Since zero is contained in the interval, the null hypothesis that μ1 μ2 = 0 cannot be rejected at the0 .05 level since zero is one of the plausible values of μ1 - μ2. The interval contains both positive and negative numbers and therefore μ1 may be either larger or smaller than μ2. None of the three possible relationships between μ1 and μ2: μ1 - μ2 = 0, μ1 - μ2 > 0, and μ1 - μ 2 < 0 can be ruled out. The data are very inconclusive. Whenever a significance test fails to reject the null hypothesis, the direction of the effect (if there is one) is unknown. Now, consider the 95% confidence interval: 6 ≤ μ1 - μ2 ≤ 15. Since zero is not in the interval, the null hypothesis that μ1 - μ2 = 0 can be rejected at the 0.05 level. Moreover, since all the values in

the interval are positive, the direction of the effect can be inferred: μ1 > μ2. Whenever a significance test rejects the null hypothesis that a parameter is zero, the confidence interval on that parameter will not contain zero. Therefore either all the values in the interval will be positive or all the values in the interval will be negative. In either case, the direction of the effect is known.

Related Documents


More Documents from "vijay srivastav"