Tagalog Structures Word 2003 Format

  • November 2019
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Tagalog Structures Word 2003 Format as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 7,608
  • Pages: 40
Overview of Tagalog Sentence Structures1: Minimalist Approach

Resty M. Cena Universidad ng Pilipinas-Diliman [email protected]

PART I: THE BASICS In Part I: The Basics, we present the basic terminology of the minimalist methodology. Examples are in Tagalog.

The Minimalist Approach Current minimalist efforts strive to achieve two independent but intertwined results: methodological minimalism and substantive minimalism. Scientific inquiry in general aims for methodological minimalism: a relentless drive to find the simplest, most elegant, and most general descriptive devices. In this tradition, the Minimalist Approach keeps the descriptive apparatus to the minimum. Structures follow a single mold: the x-bar schema. Two major structural relations are recognized: Contain and C-command. And only one syntactic operation: Merge (Move is considered a type of Merge). The whole enterprise is constrained by universal principles and parameters. The desideratum of substantial minimalism is motivated by the commitment to the idea that the language system is an optimal system for the condition the system was created, and that is mainly to generate structures intelligible to the semantic and phonetic form components. Minimalism is the second and current approach within the Principles and Parameters approach. Preceding Minimalism was Government and Binding theory.

1

Lecture notes in Linguistics 140, second semester, school year 2006-2007.

Our objective in this study is to bring to fore properties of Tagalog that have the potential to contribute to the clarification of issues in minimalist studies. Going in the other direction, we will use the minimalist framework on Tagalog data to elucidate some of the ways the language works.

The X-Bar Schema The x-bar schema is a fixed representation of the structure of phrases. It is one of the ways that structures intra- and inter-languages are constrained from exhibiting too much variation. The X-bar schema: XP Specifier

/ X’ / \ X Complement

Looking at the x-bar schema above, we say that •

X is the head of the structure. It is the minimal projection of X itself.



X’ (x-bar) is an intermediate projection of X, it is a structure that is larger than a word and smaller than a phrase.



XP is the maximal projection of X; it is a projection X that is not contained within any larger constituent with the same head X.



The specifier and the complement are maximal projections.



Specifier is a phrase that carries some grammatical function in respect of X.



Ccomplement is a phrase that complements X.



The head X dictates the properties of the whole phrase.

The example below shows the x-bar structure of the prepositional phrase talagang para diyan. The items match one-for-one the items in the schema above.

Talagang para diyan PP / ADV specifier of para talaga +ng

P’ / P

The adverb talaga serves as

\ PRN

complement of para. para

The pronoun diyan serves as

diyan

PP = prepositional phrase, P = preposition, ADV = adverb, PRN = pronoun Classic X-bar theory is under scrutiny, as most everything else, in current minimalist efforts.

Creating Structures: Merge Derivation is carried out using merge. Merge merges two constituents at a time to form a single constituent at the next higher level. Let us show the derivation of the phrase talagang para diyan. Talagang para diyan PP / ADV

P’

form PP talaga+ng

/ P

Step 2: Merge P’ and ADV to

\ PRN

Step 1: Merge P and PRN to form

P’ para

diyan

1. First, we merge the preposition P para and its complement, the pronoun diyan, to form the (small) prepositional phrase P’ para diyan. 2. Then we merge the resulting structure P’ with the specifier adverb ADV talaga+ng to form the prepositional phrase PP talagang para diyan. The structure PP consists of two sub-structures, P’ and ADV. P’ in turn consists of two sub-structures, P and PRN. That a syntactic structure may consist of two and only two structures is guaranteed by the Binarity Principle.

BINARITY PRINCIPLE2 Every syntactic structure is binary-branching. The structures P’ and PP in the diagram above are binary-branching. HEADEDNESS PRINCIPLE Every syntactic structure is a projection of a head word. The structures P’ and PP in the diagram above are projections of the head word para.

Structural Relations: Contain and C-Command The minimalist approach recognizes two structural relations: contain and ccommand. C ONTAIN A constituent X contains another constituent Y if there is a direct path from X to Y and X is higher up in the structure than Y. Talagang para diyan PP

PP contains all structures underneath

it. / ADV

P’

structure. talaga +ng

ADV does not contain any

/ \ P

PRN

P’ contains P and

PRN. para

diyan

Hence, PP contains all the constituents underneath PP. P’ contains the constituents P and PRN. In terms of nodes of the tree diagram, a mother node is a container, a daughter node a containee, and constituents X and Y are sister nodes if both are immediate daughters of the same mother node. C-COMMAND A constituent c-commands its sister and all its constituents. 2

Definitions are from Radford, Andrew. 1994. Minimalist Syntax: Exploring the Structure of English. Cambridge University Press.

Thus P’ c-commands ADV because ADV is its sister. Regular c-command is a symmetrical relation (an anti-symmetrical c-command is recognized). Hence, ADV ccommands P’ because P’ is a sister, and ADV also c-commands P and PRN because they are daughters of P’, which is a constituent that ADV c-commands. Neither P nor PRN c-commands ADV because ADV is not a sister nor is it a constituent under a constituent that P’ c-commands. Talagang para diyan PP commanding

PP has no sister, hence, is not in a c/

ADV P’ they c-command each talaga +ng / \ P PRN because they are para diyan c-commands.

position. P’ and ADV are sisters, hence, other ADV c-commands P and PRN daughters of P’, which ADV

Features Lexical items at the end of the branches of the tree include grammatical features. Functional items can simply be a bundle of grammatical features. The orthographic forms we see on the page are merely convenient labels. For example, the pronoun sila will enter into the derivation with at least the following features. P’ /

\ PRN sila [3-Per] [pl-Nbr] [u-Case]

3rd person plural number unvalued case

Agreement relations are expressed using features. Features are checked to steer derivations in the right direction: a representation legible to both the semantic and phonetic interpretations.

Lexical Phrases Lexical phrases are headed by a lexical item (as opposed to functional item). Some common lexical phrases:

DP PP VP AP NP

Head determiner phrase determiner preposition phrase preposition verb phrase verb root adjective phrase adjective root noun phrase noun

lahat ng panday talagang para sa bata bumili ng basi sabik sa anak tao sa bukid

Determiner Phrase (DP) The determiner phrase is headed by a determiner. Noun phrases serve as complement, and quantifier phrases and adjectival phrases typically serve as specifier.. lahat ng mga panday

/

DP \

D’ QP / \ lahat D NP ng mga panday

Quantifier phrase serves as specifier Determiner ng is head of the phrase

The set of determiner consists of the particles ang, ng, sa, si, ni, kay, and their plural counterparts. Later, we will look more closely at the range of structures that come under DP, for example: ang mga panday sa malaking bahay niya sa kay Ben na bahay ang mga panday na gumagawa ng kanyon sa gumagawa ng kanyon na mga panday

Preposition Phrase (PP) A preposition heads the prepositional phrase. Transitive prepositions show complements. ADV elements serve as specifier. Talagang para diyan PP / P’

\ ADV

/ P para

\

talaga +ng DP sa bata

Some other words that may be used in place of para in the above are: doon, diyan, dine, buhat, galing, tungo, … Most of these have longer forms: nandoon, nandiyan, nandine, naroroon, naririyan, naririne, … The last three forms mentioned carry CVr of the type that verbs use. Others like buhat, galling, and tungo show forms that indicate states: nagbuhat, nanggaling, patungo. Many forms we call preposition may well support a verb analysis. Even the quintessential preposition para allows a verb form.

Adjective Phrase (AP) The adjective phrase is headed by the adjective root. higit na sabik kaysa sa panday AP /

\

A’ /

ADV higit + na

\

A DP structure inside an x-bar sabik / \ D’ Comparative / \ kaysa D N sa panday

A fully extended x-bar structure

The diagram may appear complex, but it is simply our basic x-bar structure (AP), whose head takes a complement that is a fully specified x-bar structure (DP). Complex structures are hierarchies of x-bar structures.

Verb Phrase (VP) The verb phrase is headed by the root of the verb. madalas bumili ng basi. VP /

\

V’ / \ V bili

ADV madalas DP ng basi

The affix –um- is not part of VP. It is head of another projection that we will discuss later.

Noun Phrase (NP) The noun head of a noun phrase may have an attributive phrase as specifier and a PP as complement.. NP / \ N’ AP / \ kalbo+ng N PP tao sa buwan

Sentences English main sentences have a verb as head of the predicate. It is the finite tense in the verb that is responsible for a subject expression. He read the book. TP / PRN he

T’ /

\

T [past-Tense]

Tense Phrase

/ V read

V’ \ DP the book

Given a finite verb, an English sentence must have a subject. This is captured in the following generalization. EXTENDED PROJECTION PRINCIPLE (for English) A finite Tense constituent must be extended into a TP projection that containing a subject. The following are the challenges of Tagalog to EPP.

• • •

Given that Tagalog verbs show aspect rather than tense, which element of a verbal phrase serve as head? There are many non-verbal sentences (for example, Mabait si Ben, Panday si Ben, atbp) . What is the head of these sentences? There are many sentences without a subject. Do they violate the Extended Projection Principle?

Sentence Types in Tagalog Tagalog sentences may be classified according to the type of their predicates. Verbal Bumili si Ben ng basi. Adjectival Magalang si Ben. Nominal Panday si Ben. Prepositional Para kay Ben ang basi. Existential May pera si Ben Occasionally Time and Measurement predicates are sometimes considered to be separate sentence types. More commonly, the time phrase is analyzed as a prepositional phrase, and the measurement phrase an adjective phrase. Time Sa Linggo ang pasko. Measurement Tatlo ang dumating.

Sentences with a Verbal Predicate: Voice Affix as Head The head of a verbal sentence is the voice affix. To indicate the head of the verbal sentence, we use the same symbol T as used in English, and we call it Tinig “voice”. Bumili si Benng basi. Binili ni Ben ang basi.

The voice suffix –in is unrealized.

Bumili ng basi si Ben T’ /

T is voice “tinig”. \

T VP specifies the Voice of the verb. um / \ V’ DP / \ si Ben V DP bili ng basi

T –um- , not the root bili,

With only an aspect affix, the verb has no subject, thus: Kabibili ni Ben ng basi. Kabibili ni Ben ?ang basi. Furthermore, pag-nominalizations may show aspect and mode, but it is the lack of voice that conditions an unselected subject. ang pag-iinom ni Ben ng basi ang pagpakuha ni Ben ng mga

CVr - is aspect; no voice, no subject affix pa- is mode; no voice no subject

Later on we will show that T carries a feature that, when satisfied, displaces the agent si Ben to subject position.

Sentences with an Adjective Predicate: Adjectivalizer Affix as Head The essence of adjectival sentences is the conferment of an attribute, expressed mainly by the adjective head of the predicate, to the subject. The adjective is an attribute term through ma- affixation on a base. The head T of the adjectival sentence is Tangi (from katangian) “attribute”, realized in many adjectives as ma-. Mabilis si Ben. TP /

\

T’ /

DP si Ben

\

T

Tangi (from katangian) Phrase

AP

“tangi” ma

/ A bilis

The adjectivalizer ma- serves as T

\ ADV talaga + ng

The prefix ma- requires a subject. Compare the above sentence with adjectival sentences below, which don’t carry the ma- adjective affix, and which are subjectless. Kabilis ni Ben. Napakabilis ni Ben. In the presence of a ma- affix, a subject is required, thus: Pinakamabilis si Ben.

A few bare adjectives, for example, pandak, pangit, pikon, etc., don’t accept the ma- affix, thus, *mapandak, *mapangit, *mapikon, but show a subject. Pandak si Ben. Pangit si Ben.

*Mapandak si Ben. *Mapangit si Ben.

The ma- affix shows up in these adjectives in certain constructions, for example : Mapandak-pandak si Ben. Mapait-pait ang gamot

*Pandak-pandak si Ben *Pait-pait ang gamot.

Sentences with a Locative Predicate: Locative Affix is Head The locative marker ‘affix’ na- may also serve as head of the predicate phrase. Talagang naroon sa bukid ang trabaho. TP /

Tukoy (from pantukoy) Phrase \

T’ / T na/ P doon

QP ang trabaho

\ PP / \ P’ \

The ‘affix’ na- serves as T

ADV talaga + ng

DP sa bukid

It would appear that PP alone cannot serve as predicate. One potential exception is a PP introduced by para, as in the sentence: Para sa bata ang pansit. Other examples: Nasa bukid si Ben. Nasa kay Ben ang pera. Nakay Ben ang pera.

Sentences with an Existential Predicate: Head is Existential Particle The head of an existential sentence is the existential particle may.

May pera si Ben. T’ /

\

T may

VP / \ V’ DP / \ si Ben V DP Ø pera

Other examples: May pera. May pera kay Ben. May pera kay Obet si Ben. Mayroong pera kay Ben.

Sentences with a Nominal Predicate: Head is the Nominalizer Affix Many Tagalog nominals are non-derived, for example, panday, sundalo, bundok, atbp. But many are derived, for example, manggagamot, mangingisda, tagaluto, tagabantay, atbp. Bare nominals as predicate Panday si Ben. Sundalo si Ben. Derived nominals as predicate Tagapanday si Ben. Manggagamot si Ben. Tagagamot si Ben. The following diagram shows the nominlizing affix taga- as head. .

Tagapanday si Ben. T’ /

\

T taga

VP /

\

The noun-forming affix taga serves as

T V panday

DP si Ben

As for sentences with bare nouns as predicate, a null T variant of the nominalizer taga is proposed. The absence of an affix is what distinguishes pure nouns from adjectives and verbs. Panday si Ben.

cf. Tagapanday si Ben.

T’ / T Ø

\

/ V panday

VP \ DP si Ben

taga- attaches successfully with many bare nouns, thus: tagapanday, tagadoktor, tagapangulo, taganars, tagapulis, etc., though not with many others, such as, *tagapresidente, *taga-senador, *taga-sundalo. Ricardo Nolasco (pers. comm.) suggests that in constructions like Panday si Ben, panday is attributive rather than referential, hence, it is not a nominal expression.

Other Non-Derived Predicate Heads In addition to bare adjectives and bare nouns, other word classes may serve as head of the predicate phrase. Bukas ang Pasko. Tatlo ang dumating.

bukas is a time word tatlo is a cardinal number

Time phrases are likely to be prepositional phrases. Sa kinabukasan pa ang Pasko. Sa Linggo ang Pasko.

Noong Linggo ang Pasko. Measurement phrases are likely to be truncated noun phrases: Tatlo ang dumating.

cf. Tatlong tao ang dumating.

Summary Heads of predicate phrases are typically affixes on the head word of the predicate. This approach succeeds in constructions where the head word has the following affixes: Voice affix Adjectivalizer maLocative affix naNominalizer taga-, mang-

Uminom si Ben. Malakas si Ben. Naroon sa bukid si Ben. Tagagawa si Ben, Manggagawa si Ben.

The head of an existential phrase is the particle may. Existential

May pera si Ben.

For bare nominal heads, we proposed a null variant of taga-, and for bare adjectives a null variant of ma-, thus: Bare noun Bare adjective ma-.)

Panday si Ben. cf. Tagapanday si Ben. Pandak si Ben.

(Head is null variant of taga-.) (Head is null variant of

cf. Mapandak-pandak si Ben. Time expressions (e.g. Sa lingo ang pista.) are PPs if not DPs.

Subject as Specifier Where do subjects come from?

Subject as Specifier of VP It is standard practice in minimalist analysis to locate the agent phrase (which is the default subject phrase in English and other languages) initially in the specifier position of VP (spec-VP), as shown in the diagram below. Predicate-Internal Argument Hypothesis All the arguments of a predicate originate within a projection of the predicate.

As shown below, si Ben is under VP and within the projection of the verb bili. Bumili si Ben ng basi T’ /

\

T um

VP / \ V’ DP projection of V into spec-VP / \ si Ben V DP bili ng basi

si Ben originates as

Subject as Specifier of TP On the above structure, EPP applies and copies si Ben into spec-TP, as shown in the diagram below. The strikethrough on the original si Ben indicates that the phrase has been copied. TP /

\

\

DP si Ben

T’ /

T VP [EPP] / \ um V’ DP / \ si Ben V DP bili ng basi

“Copy / Move” creates structures, in this case, TP. It is a form of merge, since it creates a structure by merging two structures, in this case the constituent structures T’ and DP. The constituent structures originate inside the tree, hence it is called internal merge. In contrast, regular merge joins structures coming from the lexicon, hence, it is called external merge. This process is called spec-VP-to-Spec-TP movement. It is an example of Amovement, so called because movement is to an argument position.

The Predicate-Internal Argument Hypothesis makes it easy to map structural and thematic arguments. For example, the DP specifier maps to the agent role, and the DP complement to the patient role. The feature [EPP] triggers the extension of the head um to a TP projection, and spec-VP raises to spec-TP. The Attract Closest Principle insures that EPP moves the right constituent. ATTRACT CLOSET PRINCIPLE (ACP) A head which attracts a given kind of constituent attracts the closest constituent of the relevant type which it c-commands. In our example, T matches the DP si Ben. (Matching is explained under the section on Agreement). When the verb carries the patient voice affix, it is the complement basi and not the agent Ben that raises (note that the determiners ang and ni are only convenient labels used in place of the set of grammatical features representing those determiner positions). How does Attract Closest know which is the relevant nominal to operate on? Case marking, which we discuss under Agreement, could help. Binili ni Ben ang basi.

(Verb carries the unrealized –in voice affix) TP / T’ / T [EPP] in V bili

\ DP ang basi

\ VP

/ V’ / \

\ DP ni Ben DP ang basi

Raising Another example of A-movement is subject raising. In the examples below, the italicized subject siya is raised outside of the embedded clause and into the main sentence. Pinilit ko [na kumain] Pinilit ko [na kumain siya]

Below is the derivation of the second sentence. TP /

\

T’ /

\ T VP in / \ V’ / \ V CP pilit / \ C na

PRN siya

PRN ko

TP

/ T’ kumain

\ PRN siya

More examples: Pinilit ko ang sarili ko [na kumain] Pinilit ko siya [na kumain]

Subjectless Sentences Tagalog has many sentences with no ang-phrase . Since we consider here the angphrase as the subject phrase, these sentences would appear to be subjectless.

Recent Perfective Sentences In recent perfective sentences, the predicate head has the form: ka + CV + stem, where CV is the reduplication of the first CV of the stem. Kabibili Kabibili Kabibili Kabibili

ni Ben ng basi. *si Ben ng basi. ni Ben ?ang basi. ni Ben sa basi.

Kabibili ni Ben ng basi. Asp’ / \ Asp VP ka + bi +bili / \

Aspect (small) phrase

V’ DP / \ ni Ben V DP bili ng basi Now, ka- is not a voice affix, it is the head of an aspect phrase. The Asp head is weak and cannot copy Ben to serve as its specifier.

Intensive Sentences The intensive predicate head has the form: napaka + adjective stem. Notice, as shown in the third and fourth examples below, that the adjective form must not carry the ma- adjectivalizer affix. Napakasuwerte ni Ben. Napakasuwerte *si Ben. *Napakamasuwerte ni Ben. *Napakamasuwerte si Ben. The following are variants of the first two sentences above: (Napa)kasuwerte (Napa)kasuwerte (Na)pakasuwerte (Na)pakasuwerte

ni Ben. *si Ben. ni Ben. si Ben.

Napakasuwerte ni Ben. Intense’ / \ Intense AP napaka / \ A’ / A suwerte

Intensifier (small) phrase

DP ni Ben

As with the Focus head, the Intense head is too weak to copy ni Ben to the specifier position. Hence, the sentence ends up subjectless. Interestingly, superlative adjective predicates have subjects. Pinakamasuwerte si Ben. Pinakamasuwerte *ni Ben.

*Pinakasuwerte si Ben. *Pinakasuwerte ni Ben. The difference between intensified (e.g., napakasuwerte) and superlative (e.g., pinakamasuwerte) predicates is that the former has a null T (that is, no maadjectivalizer) while the later does. Pinakamasuwerte si Ben. TP / T’ / T

\ DP \ si Ben Super’

Superlative (small)

phrase ma + pinaka + suwerte / \ Super AP pinaka + suwerte / \ A DP suwerte si Ben As with the heads Focus and Intense, Super is weak and is unable to copy si Ben to serve as its specifier. However, during the merging of the head T ma- and Super’, the head ma-, possessing a strong feature, is able to copy-raise si Ben to specifier. Thus, EPP predicts the presence of a subject in Pinakamasuwerte si Ben and its absence in Napakasuwerte ni Ben.

Exclamatives Exclamatives don’t show a subject phrase (we don’t consider the ang-phrase, ang likot and ang takbo in the examples below as subject). Ang likot ni Ben. Ang takbo ni Ben.

cf. Ang likot *si Ben. cf. Ang takbo *si Ben.

These sentences appear structurally more like a noun phrase with a possessor, thus: Ang likot ni Ben. DP / D’

\ DP

/ D ang

\

ni Ben

N likot

Sentences with Affixless Verbs Sentences with the affixless predicates gusto, ayaw, nais, and ibig do not show a subject when the patient is indefinite. Gusto ni Ben ng basi. Ayaw ni Ben ng gin. These sentences do not have a head item that can trigger the creation of a new structure above VP that can host a subject. Gusto ni Ben ng basi. VP / V’ / V gusto

\ \

DP ni Ben

DP ng basi

When the patient noun is definite, these affixless verbs require a subject. Gusto ni Ben ang basi. Ayaw ni Ben ang gin. We will attempt to explain these facts in a later section using the concept of “feature movement” (Chomsky 1995). The verb gusto and its synonyms and antonym may, of course, be used like any regular verb, that is, with verbal affixes. These sentences derive normally, with the voice affixes serving as T. Ginusto ni Ben si Gina. Umayaw si Ben sa basi.

Subjectless Weather Verbs Weather verbs inflect for voice, but, nonetheless don’t show a subject, apparently in contradiction of EPP which guarantees that a Tagalog voiced verb will have a subject..

Umulan. Umulan sa Maynila. The structure of the second example is as follows: Umulan sa Maynila. T’ / \ T um [EPP] / V ulan

VP / V’ \ DP sa Maynila

EPP applies and attempts to extend T to a projection with a subject. However, there is no appropriate constituent that can serve as subject. Weather verbs show a subject if the verb is inflected for the patient voice (not the locative voice, even though the subject is a location), as shown below. In this case, the voice affix –in (unrealized in the verb inulan) serves as T, and the sentence is derived normally. Inulan ang Maynila. When weather-verbs are used metaphorically, they carry T and are derived in the usual way. Umulan si Ben ng kasinungalingan Inulan kami ng kasinungaligan ni Ben. So the absence of subject in Umulan and Umulan sa Maynila may simply be because there just isn’t any nominal available to serve as specifier of –um-.

Subjectless Existential Sentences Some forms of the existential sentences do not show a subject. May pera. May pera kay Ben.

(No other argument except the existent.) (Second argument is locative-marked.)

May nakatagong pera kay Ben. Ben.

cf. May nakatagong pera si Obet kay

One could say that these sentences do not show a verb, thus: May pera kay Ben. TP / T’ / T [EPP] may

\

VP / \ V’ DP / \ kay Ben V DP Ø pera

EPP applies but is unable to find the right expression, hence, nothing moves to specTP. One apparent anomaly with existential sentences is that when the existent is a verb form, it must carry a non-agent affix, this despite the fact that the argument is interpreted as agent. May biniling basi si Ben. affix. May *bumiling basi si Ben. unacceptable. May biniling basi si Ben.

Ben is agent, but binili has a non-agent voice An agent voice affix renders the sentence

CP / C’ /

\ DP si Ben

\

C [EPP, EXIST] may

TP / \ T’ DP / \ si Ben T VP [EPP] / \ -in + bili V’ DP / \ si Ben V DP bili basi

One possible explanation is that binili is not a verb but a noun, hence, may binili is a nominal expression, in the same sense that ang binili is a nominal. The difference is that the [EXIST] feature in T of an may binili si Ben ang binili ni Ben CP /

\

C’ / \

DP si Ben

C TP [EPP, EXIST] / \ Ø T’ DP / \ si Ben T D’ Ø / \ D N may binili This is questionable since this is the only instance of a possessor nominal (si Ben) in the nominative rather than in the usual genitive form. Nawala ang may binili. Nawala ang may binili *si Ben.

Sentences with ‘Double Subjects’ Complementiser Phrase Complementiser phrases are head by the complementiser na, kung, kaya, atbp.

Dependent Clause as CP Consider the following : Sinabi ni Ben [na umuwi ka]. The dependent clause na umuwi ka consists of the complementiser na and its complement, the TP umuwi ka. The whole CP serves as complement of the verb sabi. Sinabi ni Ben na umuwi ka. T’ / \ T in

/

VP \

V’ / \ V sabi

C na

DP ni Ben CP

/ C’ / \ TP umuwi ka

Independent Clause as CP It is convenient to regard independent clauses to be CPs as well. Independent clauses typically do not show an overt complementiser phrase.

Hinagip ang pusa. CP / C’ \

/ C Ø

T’ / \ T in

complementiser head is null VP

/ V’ / \

V hagip

DP ang pusa

A bit of support is provided by sentences introduced by exclamatives, where the exclamatives may be considered to serve as C. Ay, hinagip ang pusa! Uy, nakabago tayo. Ay, hinagip ang pusa C’ \

/ C ay

TP /

\

T’ / T in V hagip

DP ang pusa

\ /

V’ \ DP ang pusa

The specifier of CP (spec-CP) is a position, unrealized in the above example, that will be the locus of movement, as we will see shortly. All clauses are CPs.

Movement Earlier we have seen the raising of a nominal to the subject position, an example of A-movement, which moves a constituent to an argument position. We discuss here other types of movement processes.

V-to-C Movement Affixes occupy a strong position, and can attract a host to attach to. In the sentence below the affix um attracts the verb bili. Phonetic rules will give the spell-out bumili. This process is called V-to-T movement and is an example of head-to-head movement, so called because the constituent that moves is a head and its destination is a head position.

Bumili ng basi si Ben T’ / T um + bili

\ VP / V’ / \

V bili

\ DP si Ben VP ng basi

DEFINITION OF ATTRACT To say that a head H attracts a constituent C is to say that H triggers movement of C to some position on the edge of HP (so that C may move to adjoin to H, or to become the specifier of H). The constituent C must be the closest head that H c-commands. Move is a two-step process: copy then delete. In the diagram above the strikethrough on the original bili shows that it has been copied.

Focalisation Focalization moves an item outside of TP. Semantically the preposed constituent receives extra emphasis. Sa bukid uminom si Ben sa bukid. Sa bukid ay uminom si Ben sa bukid. We analyze the third example as follows: Si Ben ay uminom.

CP / C’ / \

\ DP si Ben

[EPP] in C moves spec-TP si Ben to

Spec-CP. C [EPP] ay

TP / T’

\ DP

[EPP] in T moves spec-VP si Ben to

spec-TP. /

\

si Ben

T VP [EPP] / \ um + inom V’ DP / \ si Ben V DP inom sa bukid For now, ay is analyzed as a complementiser. Later, when we split CP, it will serve as head of FocP (focus phrase). More examples: *Si Ben, uminom. Si Ben, uminom siya.

Topicalization In topicalization, the preposed constituent represents old information, or an expression that has been established earlier in the discourse Tanong: Aalis na sina Obet. E, Sina Ben? Sagot: Sina Ben, nauna na. We will analyse this sentence type under Split Projections.

Clefting In a cleft sentence, the nominative expression is preposed and separated from the rest of the sentence with the ang particle. Speaker A: Pulis sa baryo si Ben. Speaker B: (Oo), si Ben ang pulis sa baryo.

(cleft sentence)

We treat clefting as focalisation, where ang is the head of the projection, and where si Ben in the above sentence is the focus phrase.

Si Ben ang pulis sa baryo. FocP / \ Foc’ DP / \ si Ben Foc DP Ø / \ D NP ang / \ N’ DP / \ si Ben N DP pulis sa baryo

More examples: Si Si Si Si

Ben Ben Ben Ben

ang siyang pulis sa baryo. ay ang pulis sa baryo. ito. ay ito.

Wh-Movement Wh refers to question words: sino, ano, kailan, saan, bakit, kangin, etc. Sino and ano questions the nominative expression Ano ang binili ni Ben? Sino ang bumili ng basi?

cf. Binili ni Ben ang basi. cf. Bumili si Ben ng basi.

The rest are used to question non-nominative expressions. Saan lumangoy si Ben? Kailan umuwi si Ben? Bakit madalas ang ulan? Kangino lumapit si Ben?

cf. Lumangoy si Ben si ilog. cf. Umuwi si Ben kahapon. cf. Madalas umulan dahil tag-ulan. cf. Lumapit si Ben kay Obet.

Wh-question is represented as the feature [WH] on question words and the C head of an interrogative sentence. This feature attracts the closest item that C ccommands that also has the [WH] feature, in the example below, the PP saan, and raises it to the C position.

Saan lumangoy si Ben. C’ / C

\

[WH] saan

TP \

/ T’ /

T um + langoy / langoy

DP si Ben

\ V’ / V

\ PP [WH] saan

In the next example, the question word sino ends up in the specifier position of CP. Ano ang binili ni Ben? CP / C’ /

\

C [EPP, WH] ano as goal and raises it Ø

DP ano

\ DP \

/ D ang

C as probe finds the spec-TP TP /

T’ /

\

to become spec-CP \ DP ano

T

VP [EPP, WH] / \ in V’ DP / \ ni Ben V DP bili [WH] ano At the highest C level of derivation, [EPP] needs to be satisfied, hence, ACP looks for the closest c-commanded nominal that has a [WH] feature, and that is the DP that is specifier of TP. Wh-movement is an example of A-bar movement, which moves an argument or an adjunct expression to an argument position.

Examples to think about: Sino ang ano ang binili? Ano ang sino ang bumili?

Successive Move What appears to be ‘long-distance’ movement is actually an instance of successive local movements. In the examples below, the subject si Pat of the lowest sentence moves successively to the next higher sentence until it reaches the matrix sentence.

Sinabi Sinabi Sinabi Sinabi

ni ni ni ni

Ben Ben Ben Ben

na ibinalita ni Obet na itinatuwa ni Ming na umuwi si Pat. na ibinalita ni Obet na itinatuwa ni Ming si Pat na umuwi. na ibinalita ni Obet si Pat na itinatuwa ni Ming na umuwi. si Pat na ibinalita ni Obet na itinatuwa ni Ming na umuwi.

Agreement Agreement is a relationship between a probe and a goal. The head of a projection is probe and a c-commanded expression is goal. When a head merges with its complement, and the head contains unvalued features, the head probes its complement for a c-commanded goal that has matching features. In the following examples, the verb is marked for an affix that requires a plural subject, hence the plural pronoun sila but not the singular pronoun siya can serve as subject. Nagsikuha sila ng mga lapis. Nagsikuha *siya ng lapis.

(Verb a ffix nagsi- requires a plural subject)

Function words like the particle ng and the pronoun sila as well as affixes like nagsido not go into merging already spelled out as such, but as bundles of grammatical features. They are given spell-out in the PF component. We use these spellings in our discussion merely as convenient labels. Let us briefly discuss feature valuation.

Feature Valuation Features are either valued or unvalued as they enter derivation. Consider the following dialogue. Sila refers back to nina Ben, hence, it is known beforehand that it carries plural number and third person. Likewise, since the event happened in the past, the affix nagsiyou are here Tanong: Ano ang ginawa nina Ben? Sagot: Nagsikuha sila ng mga lapis. In the derivation shown below, as the pronoun sila enters the derivation, it carries the features plural number [pl-Nbr] and third person [3-Pers]. Its case is still unvalued, as it can turn out either sila or nila. The affix magsi- carries unvalued number and person features, and a valued case feature. By the EPP convention, T carries the [EPP] feature.

T’ / T [EPP] [nom-Case] [u-Pers] [u-Nbr] nagsi VP / V’ kuha ng lapis

\ PRN [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] [u-Case] sila

Now the head T magsi- merges with VP kuha ng lapis to form V’. T agreement applies at this point, and the probe nagsi- finds a match for its uninterpretable features matches with Agreement involves a c-command relation between a probe and a goal. Unvalued φ-features on the probe are valued by the goal, and unvalued case feature on the goal is valued by the probe. The highest head, in this case, T, serves as probe. It searches for a c-commanded goal with an unvalued case feature, and locates the pronoun sila, and an agreement relation is established between nagsi- and sila. Now the unvalued person and number features of nagsi- are valued by the goal sila, involving a Feature-Copying operation, defined as follows, where α and β are two different constituents contained within the same structure and where one is a probe and the other a goal: FEATURE-COPYING If α is valued for some feature [F] and β is unvalued for [F] and if β agrees with α, the feature-value for [F] on α is copied onto β. As a result of this operation, the person and number features of nagsi- are valued to 3rd and plural, respectively.

T’ / T [EPP] [nom-Case] [3-Pers] of the probe [pl-Nbr] nagsi

Values of underlined features are copied from … VP /

\

V’ kuha ng lapis

PRN [3-Pers]

… values of similar features of

the goal [pl-Nbr] [u-Case] sila The unvalued case feature of PRN sila is copied from the valued nominative case feature of T nagsi-, thus: T’ / T [EPP] [nom-Case] copied to … [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] nagsi

Value of a bolded feature

VP / V’ kuha ng lapis

\ PRN [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] [nom-Case] sila

… underlined feature

The [EPP] feature of T will then raise the PRN sila to the specifier of TP.

Uninterpretable Features Items may enter derivation with some of their features already valued and others as yet unvalued. Valued and unvalued features correlate with interpretable and uninterpretable features. Interpretable features play a role in semantic interpretation, and uninterpretable features do not contain any meaning that the semantic component (SC) will need to interpret. The pronoun sila in the above diagram requires interpretable person and number features because a first person pronoun like ako is clearly different from 2nd person ikaw; similarly pronouns that differ in number will have different interpretation. Nominals enter derivation with unvalued case because whether a nominal is nominative or accusative has no effect on meaning. By contrast, the person / number features of Ts are uninterpretable in that they serve purely to mark agreement with a particular nominal. The case feature of T, however, is interpretable since case carries modality in many constructions in Tagalog (Nolasco 2003), and it is important for SC to be so informed. FEATURE VALUE CORRELATION (i) Interpretable features enter the derivation already valued. (II) Features which enter the derivation unvalued are uninterpretable. An item with an uninterpretable feature is active. Only an active item may serve as probe or goal.

Illegible Features Structures generated by the syntactic component are sent to the PF component of the grammar for spell out. The PF component cannot process an unvalued feature, and derivation will crash. Thus every unvalued feature must be valued in the course of the derivation. Structures are also sent to the semantic component where they are converted into appropriate semantic interpretations. Uninterpretable features will appear illegible to the semantic component, and derivation will crash. To avoid this, derivation will mark uninterpretable features as deleted, in the sense that they will be invisible to the semantic component while remaining visible to the syntax and the PF component. FEATURE VISIBILITY CONVENTION Any uninterpretable feature deleted in the syntax is invisible to the semantic component, but remains visible in the syntactic component and in the PF component. FEATURE DELETION CONVENTION

α deletes any uninterpretable (person/number/case) feature(s) carried by β if α is phi-complete and if the value(s) of any phi-feature(s) carried by β match those of the corresponding phi-feature(s) of α. α and β are two different constituents contained within the same structure, and one is a probe and the other a goal. For β to delete the person/number/case features of α, the phi-features of β must match the phi-features carried by α. THE RELATION ‘MATCH’ α and β match in respect of some feature [F] either if both have the same value for [F], or if one is valued for [F] and the other unvalued for [F] – but not if they have different values for [F]. Consider our previous example: Nagsikuha sila ng lapis. T’ / T [EPP] [nom-Case] [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] nagsi VP / V’ kuha ng lapis

\ PRN [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] [nom-Case] sila

Both nagsi- and sila are φ-complete. φ-complete nagsi- can delete the uninterpretable case feature carried by sila, and similarly φ-complete sila can delete the uninterpretable person and number features of nagsi-. The deleted items are the features in strikethrough in the diagram below.

T’ / T [EPP] [nom-Case] [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] nagsi VP / V’ kuha ng lapis

\ PRN [3-Pers] [pl-Nbr] [nom-Case] sila

FEATURE INACTIVATION HYPOTHESIS An uninterpretable feature becomes inactive in the syntax (and invisible to the semantic component) immediately it is deleted.

Phase Syntactic structures are built up one phase at a time. Phases are propositional in nature. They include CPs, TPs, and transitive VPs. PHASE IMPENETRABILITY CONDITION (PIC) Any goal in the (c-command) domain of a phase head is impenetrable to a probe outside the phase. Consider the sentence: Sinabi [na binugbog ni Ben si Obet] This sentence cannot be interpreted to mean Sinabi ni Ben [na binugbog si Obet]. That is to say, the verb sabi cannot serve as probe to the goal ni Ben to assign the role of agent of sabi. This is because ni Ben is outside of the c-command domain of sabi.

T’ /

\

T in

VP /

The probe –in- cannot establish an

agreement V’

relation with any expression inside

CP as / V sabi

\

the CP phase is already complete. CP

/ C’ /

C na

\ TP / T’

\

DP si Obet T VP in / \ V’ DP / ni Ben V bugbog /

\

PIC insures locality of syntactic operations. Once a phase is completely formed, a transfer operation sends the domain of the phase head, which is its complement, to the semantic component and the PF component for interpretation. Thereafter, no constituent in the domain is able to undergo any further syntactic operations. In our example above, si Obet is in the completed TP phase and cannot serve as goal to the probe –in.

Split Projections Split CPs: Force, Topic, Focus, and Finiteness Projections Earlier, we suggested that preposed constituents like si Ben in the first example below is put into focus and serves as specifier of a FocP projection. Where does FocP stand in relation to CP? Consider the second example below. Clearly the FocP si Ben is positioned between CP and TP. Si Ben ay uminom.

Sinabi ko [CP na si Ben ay [TP uminom si Ben ]] To accommodate this and other facts, CP has been split into a number of projections: ForceP, TopP (topic phrase), FocP, and InfP (infinitive phrase). Complementisers, because they specify whether a clause is declarative, interrogative, imperative or exclamative in force, are analyzed as Force markers. They serve as head of a ForceP (force phrase) projection. [na si Ben ay uminom] ForceP /

\

Force na

FocP / \ Foc’ DP / \ si Ben Foc TP ay um + inom si Ben

Another projection above TP is TopP (topic phrase). The topic phrase expresses old information. In the example below, the reference to ang mga bata is assumed to have been established earlier in the discourse. Sinabi ni Ben [na ang mga bata, kahapon ay sumakay sa kalesa ang mga bata kahapon] ForceP / Force na

\

TopP / \ Top’ DP / \ ang mga bata Top FocP Ø / \ Foc’ Adv / \ kahapon Foc TP ay um + sakay sa kalesa ang mga bata

kahapon

Still another projection above TP is FinP (finite phrase). This marks a clause as either finite or non-finite. The head Fin is performed by prepositions like para which signals a following non-finite clause. Inutusan si Ben [para bumili PRO ng basi] FinP /

\

Fin para

TP bumili PRO ng basi

When TopP and FocP are absent, ForceP and FinP syncretised onto a single head.

Split VPs: VP Shells Consider the causative sentence below: Nagpaluto si Ben ng pansit kay Obet. Causatives contain two actions: the injunction and the core action expressed in the lexical verb. In our example, the injunction is expressed in an intransitive construction on the causative morpheme pa-. The core action is expressed as a transitive construction around the lexical verb. pa- si Ben luto Obet ng pansit.

Intransitive construction with pa- as verb Transitive construction on the lexical verb luto.

An obvious way to handle this double-verb scenario is to split the usual VP component into two parts: an inner VP that expresses the core action, and an outer VP that captures the injunction. VP is split into two parts: a small vP and the big VP. vP is a projection of the causative affix pa-, and VP the projection of the lexical verb, as usual. VP serves as complement of v’. Thus, the bracketed materials in the example below, which represent the complete verbal component, are given the representation as shown. Nag[paluto si Ben ng pansit kay Obet] vP /

vP is Outer VP \

v’ / v pa

DP si Ben

\ VP / V’

VP Inner VP \ DP

/ V luto

\

kay Obet

DP ng pansit

To this structure, the head T nag- is merged, giving rise to the following structure: TP / \ T’ DP / \ si Ben T vP nag + pa + luto / \ v’ DP / \ si Ben v VP pa +luto luto ng pansit kay Obet

As we will see in a later section, this solution works well for other constructions: Nakigapas si Ben ng palay kay Obet. Pinuno ni Ben ng palay ang kariton. Pipintahan ni Ben ang bahay ng pula. Inihalal ni Ben si Obet na pangulo.

(commitative predicate) (‘loaded with’ predicate) (resultative predicate) (double-object predicate)

Summary The above discussion should provide an understanding of elementary operations of minimalism and of basic minimalist structures in Tagalog. We will now examine more intricate structures in Tagalog,

Related Documents

Word Format
May 2020 5
Word 2003
November 2019 14
Word 2003
June 2020 12