Communication Language And The Ability To Learn

  • Uploaded by: Alan Challoner
  • 0
  • 0
  • May 2020
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Communication Language And The Ability To Learn as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 6,485
  • Pages: 11
Communication, Language and the Ability to Learn Alan Challoner MA MChS

The more complete the information that a person is able to communicate to someone he trusts, the more he himself becomes able to dwell on it, to understand it and to see its implications. John Bowlby, 1991.

Vocabulary and the general adequacy and completeness of speech vary by socioeconomic class. Speech is poorer in form and articulation, less in amount, and less precise for children at lower socio-economic levels than for those at higher social levels (see Irwin, 1948,1). Another important consideration is that the sooner a child develops adequate language, the sooner he is able to understand the intentions and requirements of his parents, and to respond accordingly. This verbal comprehension helps a child understand the difference between his “now” behaviour and his “next”. A better discrimination of his mother’s intentions leads to more efficient learning. 2 One of the early ways of predicting the future intelligence of children, tested as infants, was the measurement of their speech sounds.3 Children reared with meagre adult attention, and who are relatively isolated or who live in institutions, are handicapped even more in their speech than in their general IQ level. This type of handicap has been shown to exist as early as two months of age. Children who, at elementary school ages, test at the learning disability or borderline level started talking later than children who measured normal or bright. At least during the early years of their lives, twins and triplets test lower in both speech and intelligence than singletons, although much of this lag is overcome by school age. Their speech seems to be even more handicapped than can be accounted for by their somewhat lower intelligence test scores. Pathak, et al., have studied the influence of socialisation, parental deprivation and family psychiatric history on the speech development of 267 institutionalized learning disabled children aged ten years. The subjects were matched on sex, locality and IQ level as well as on age. The indications from the study are that where speech development takes place it has an effective quality that depends upon the social environment in which the therapy is located. Where there were lack of adequate components such as poor environment and small family size, the child became discouraged and self-centred. There was no relation found between speech development and the family psychiatric history.4 Skuse takes the view that the evidence is not clear-cut regarding the possibility that some exposure to language and communication is essential at an early age in order to facilitate later acquisition of speech.5 While the evidence is not entirely clear-cut, children who are bilingual during the pre-school yearsthat is, typically, whose homes use one language while their eventual schools use a different one test lower than monoglots. Although social class may be involved, since children from such homes are rather more frequently from lower than from upper-class homes [USA], the effect seems greater than can be accounted for by social class alone. Work on the perceptual development of language in infants has been reviewed by Cooper & Aslin.6 They have looked at the use of auditory information during early language processing, and have found evidence that indicates that infants actively process sounds, particularly those with acoustic attributes of their native language. It has been established that infants as young as four months can identify their colloquial speech when presented within a mixture of other accented speech and other languages. The mother’s voice registers characteristically with the infant, and with the fetus from about 20 weeks after conception. This possibly is related to the musical components of speech. These are:

1

metrical structure or rhythm, the melodic structure, and the timbre or voice quality. Infants’ pre-natal experience with maternal speech may determine the early post-natal perceptual salience of a specific mother’s speech, motherese speech, and native speech. Infant’s sensitivity to suprasegmental aspects of speech may dictate much of what they attend to, whilst features of infantSuprasegmental Analysis directed speech seem to Communication through speech relies on not just words draw their attention to the but how they are spoken. Speech is bound to the time speaker and elicit positive continuum, and we must receive it as it happens, moment affective responses. by moment. In order to understand it we search its form, Following the use of brain examining segments in a search for a structure that is scans, it has been discovered already laid down or is developing in our brains. The why it is easier for children to segments of spoken language are the vowels and learn a second language consonants that combine to form syllables, words and than it is for adults. sentences. When we articulate these segments our If the second language is pronunciation varies, in accordance with our tone of learned at the same time as voice. the primary, it is stored in the Components of a sound-based language same region of the brain; in Phonemes The individual sound units, whose the part of the frontal lobe concatenation, in a particular order, produces the basic known as Broca’s area. If the sounds of language and dialect called morphemes. second language is learned Certain sounds cause changes in the meaning of a word or later, it is still stored in Broca’s phrase, whilst others do not. In English there are some 40 area, but separated from the such sounds that are seen as important units, and these are first language. It seems that called phonemes or segmental phonemes. learning the primary language singly sets up neural circuits in a specific area, and these will not accommodate a laterlearned language. These findings also suggest that the age of language acquisition may be a significant factor in determining how this part of the brain is organised. Infants start by being able to recognise equally, all relevant sounds, but as they learn their native tongue, the way it is represented in Broca’s area becomes fixed.

Morphemes The smallest meaningful units of a word, whose combination creates one. In order to learn grammar a child must segment the speech he hears into morphemes, as these are the basic units of grammatical rules. There are smaller units called segmental phonemes and these form the basic sounds of language and dialect. These are put together into syllables to form morphemes. Syntax The admissible combinations of words in phrases and sentences (called grammar, in popular usage). Lexicon The collection of all words in a given language. Each lexical entry includes all information with morphological or syntactic ramifications but does not include conceptual knowledge. Semantics The meanings that correspond to all lexical items and to all possible sentences.

Prosody The vocal intonation that can modify Social class seems to be the literal meaning of words and sentences. directly related to The linking of sentences such that vocabulary level; yet children Discourse they constitute a meaningful whole. from some middle-class homes have been shown to have low vocabularies, while those from some lower-class homes have relatively good vocabularies. One author 7 provides evidence indicating why there are such exceptions (at the same time presenting data to show that the child’s language status and his socio-economic status correlate somewhere between 0.78 and 0.86, depending on which of two statistical techniques is used). The twenty-one high-scorers in this study of language status by Milner, which consisted of forty-two first-graders, came from homes ranging from upper-lower to lower-upper, this latter being the highest social class included in the study. They were distinguished as a

2

group from the twenty-one low-scorers by such factors as eating breakfast and dinner with their families and engaging in conversation during those times. Their discipline inclined more toward guidance and prohibition, whereas physical discipline was more characteristic for the low-scoring children. High-scorers were more frequently expected to look after their own possessions and/or room and received more praise and affection. More high than low-scorers had been exposed to baby sitters, and they went to bed later, frequently as late as ten p.m. Low-scorers were ‘bribed’ with small gifts of money more frequently than high-scorers. The high-scorers, as might be expected, possessed more books and indicated that their mothers and fathers read to them more frequently. More of the low-scorers said they could not recall ever feeling really happy and were less frequently able to recall instances or situations where they had felt happy. Low-scorers more frequently possessed, as reading material, only funny books and /or school books. Such factors as those listed above are, of course, strongly related to social class; but where there are variations in language within a social class, it is plausible that such variables as parental conversation, attention, praise, and so on, could produce the difference between the low and the high-scorers. DeBaryshe, et al., formulated a system for measuring parental beliefs about reading to their children. Their Parent Reading Belief Inventory was completed by 155 parents of children aged between 25 and 65 months. The Inventory’s structure formed a single factor, and high scores on this indicated a belief by the parent that was consistent with current theories of language acquisition and emergent literacy. When parent income and education were controlled, inventory scores remained significantly correlated in respect to: •

self-report measures of parents’ own book-reading habits;



children’s interest in books;



children’s exposure to joint book-reading activities.

The scores also showed a significant partial correlation with the observed frequency (during book-reading sessions) of: •

parental questions;



and responsiveness to children’s speech8

The hypothesis that parental reinforcement builds language is supported by an unpublished study of Irwin’s (1960). He secured the co-operation of a large group of mothers whose husbands were mostly skilled, semi-skilled, and unskilled labourers. It could be expected that such mothers did little reading to their children at any age and probably none in the first year or so of their babies’ lives. He persuaded fifty-five mothers to read aloud to their children for at least ten minutes a day from the time they were one year old. The participation in the study of another, control group of mothers was invited by offers to check on the development of their babies. Otherwise no change was made in the interaction between them and their children. Irwin measured the youngsters’ speech development regularly and found great differences in all phases of speech by the time the children were twenty months of age. These differences appeared to be highly significant statistically. This study is especially provocative when the relations that have been found between speech and intelligence are considered. Irwin reports the experimental mothers’ amazement and chagrined amusement: ‘You asked us to read ten minutes a day,’ some exclaimed, ‘but I can’t get away from that kid. He wants me to read to him all the time.’ There are projects being developed to foster the development of early literacy amongst low-income and ‘working-class’ families. One such took place in Australia from 1987 to 1992. In this project workers assisted families by: •

reading regularly with them;



discussing issues around the implementation of the programme such as the management of home-school relations when reaching out to disadvantaged families;



and the effects of the project on children’s literacy competence.9

Another investigation examined the connections between attachment and the frequency of reading in mother-infant dyads. A subject group of 18 children who were read to

3

infrequently were matched to a control group of 18 children who were read to daily. The mean age of the subjects was 3.4 years. The outcome established that mothers in dyads that used reading frequently did not need to put pressure on their children to focus on a set reading task as often as did those mothers of the subject group. Of the mothers whose attachment to their child was less secure it was found that they: •

spent less time reading to their child;



had more troublesome episodes during the set reading, than did the control mothers.

The researchers also presented a view that the security of the mother-child attachment was related to the mothers’ representation of their relationship with their parents.10 A longitudinal study of pre-school literacy experience and later reading achievement was carried out by Scarborough, et al., with 56 middle-class children and their 112 biological parents.11 The children were first observed during their pre-school years and then again at the end of Grade 2. The intermediate set intervals were at ages of: 30 months; 36 months; 42 months; and 48 months; at which time the children were tested and the mothers completed a questionnaire. In this the parents were asked about: •

frequencies of adult reading;



parent-child reading;



and the child’s solitary book activities;

in the home. The parental responses were compared for three groups set according to the parents’ own reading skills and their children’s reading achievement in grade 2. The outcome showed that the eventual poor readers (22) had less frequent early literacy-related experiences than did those who became better readers (34). n Vernon believes that the main function of public language is similar to that of infant speech, which is to express feeling and enhance social solidarity with the listener. It emphasises the present rather than the past or the future. It is inefficient for tracing causal relations, and incapable of providing a medium for Piaget’s formal, operational type of thinking. So, although lower social class pupils can accomplish a fair amount of mechanical learning, they are much more handicapped in attempting academic secondary/high school work. The middle-class child is familiar with both codes, and indeed uses public language quite largely in inter-personal contacts. But the lower social classes are relatively incapable of formal speech. Again the middle-class child becomes accustomed to attending to long speech sequences, whereas the lower social class child, in a noisy, incoherent environment, if anything learns not to attend.12. Theoretical contributions related to the rôle played by social class and language as factors predisposing individuals to think in a concrete manner have been made by Basil Bernstein (195813, 195914, 196015). Bernstein has attempted, in these papers, to show a relationship between two forms of linguistic expression and the manner in which relationships to objects are established. He points to the common, public or street language that is commonly used by the unskilled and semi-skilled in society. According to Bernstein, this form of language-use facilitates, “thinking of a descriptive order and sensitivity to a particular form of social interaction” (Bernstein, Idem, 1959, pp., 311). Because of this sensitivity to content, “only the simplest logical implications or boundaries of the structure will be cognized,” i.e., “certain aspects of an object will not register as meaningful cues; or, if they do, the verbal response will be inadequately determined” (Bernstein, Idem, 1958, pp., 169]. On the other hand, a formal language, the use of which is common to the middle class, …facilitates the verbal elaboration of subjective intent, sensitivity to the implications of separateness, and difference, and points to the possibilities inherent in a complex conceptual hierarchy for the organization of experience. (Bernstein, Idem, 1960, pp., 271)

The implications of this analysis are significant. Users of the public language (mainly the lower class) are predisposed to,

4

…low levels of conceptualisation an orientation to a low order of causality, a disinterest in processes, a preference to be aroused by and respond to that which is immediately given rather than to the implications of a matrix of relationships. (Bernstein, Idem, 1959, pp., 318)

McCandless considers that the empirical support of this position is weak, consisting of two minor investigations that show that the non-verbal intelligence of lower-class males in England is much higher than their verbal intelligence. He believes that if Bernstein’s analysis is correct, it could be used to afford insight into the conceptual rigidity of some persons with mental handicap. Further evidence related to this point has been provided by Mia L. Kellmer Pringle and her associates16, 17. In extensive investigations of the effects of early deprivation on the language development of children, she found that children who had resided in residential nursery schools and had experienced deprivation, “were handicapped in the formal aspects of language” (Idem 1958, pp., 285). The nursery school children “lacked the ability for verbalising fantasy and for using speech in making social relationships with contemporaries” (Idem 1958, pp., 286). Pringle believes that the results of her investigations support Goldfarbs’s assertion that deprivation is more detrimental to a child’s language development than to any other aspect of his personality. This evidence might be used to account for the low performance of culturally deprived children on verbal intelligence tests and their high levels of ability on performance type tests. n Language handicap then, is a prime factor in two respects: FIRSTLY, bilingualism of a young child who moves to a foreign country with inadequate mastery of the new language may exert a retarding influence upon intellectual development and may under certain conditions affect personality development adversely (Anastasi, 18; Arsenian, 19; Darcy, 20). A common pattern in the homes of immigrants is that the child speaks one language at home and another in school, so that his knowledge of each language is limited to certain types of situations. Inadequate facility with the language of the school interferes with the acquisition of basic concepts, intellectual skills, and information. The frustration engendered by scholastic difficulties may in turn lead to discouragement and general dislike of school. Such reactions have been noted for example by Anastasi and Cordova21. In the case of certain groups, moreover, the child’s foreign language background may be perceived by himself and his associates as a symbol of minority group status and may thereby augment any emotional maladjustment arising from such status (Spoerl22). Another view is that adequate instruction in both languages can help to eliminate some of the problems noted above. Willig contends that IQ scores for groups using different prime languages are influenced by the way that they use vocabulary. Genetic components of scores in IQ tests taken through the medium of a second language are heavily loaded with the components of that language. She points out that IQ tests rely for their outcome on language facility rather than intelligence. Thus children who are working in the environment of a second language are not in the optimal conditions for measuring their abilities.23 SECONDLY, and with far more overall effect, is the way in which language is used and learned by a child from birth onwards. Literacy can only come from the acquisition of language and both develop sequentially from the earliest beginnings of watching and listening to the mature forms used in a literate adulthood. Children who are at risk in various ways may have difficulties with the acquisition of language and literacy and early intervention is appropriate.24 This important aspect of development and intellectual stimulation is and needs to be the subject of separate studies. Briefly, at this juncture, it is worth mentioning that imitation is one of the best learning strategies that babies have. Almost uniquely amongst mammals the brain cells at birth in the human are only remotely connected. Thus brain development and neural connections occur predominantly as a result of the babies learning experiences. Crying is not only its key to survival but also to learning. Unless someone

5

(usually the mother) responds adequately and effectively to crying the baby will not survive and may well suffer in terms of learning. It can recognise its mother’s voice from three days old, and from four days can tell the difference between its native language and one that is different. Sounds can be heard and remembered by the fetus from about 20 weeks after conception. By eight weeks it prefers the sound of its native language to that of all others. At three weeks of age the baby’s most effective muscles are those of its eyes. During the pre-speech phase when a baby points to an object it is not ‘asking for it’ but it wants to know what it is called. Babies have a need to add to their growing knowledge a vital component the ability to name and classify everything around them. The more words that a child knows the faster he will learn new ones. To have a child and not to help it to acquire good language skills and develop them to their full potential is like building a house that no one ever occupies or a ship that never leaves port. Social disadvantage can preclude proper development in pre-school children. The areas that are especially vulnerable are: •

cognitive development



psycho-motor development



learning



and emotional development.

Mohan has found that lower class children are more anxious and less motivated. He believes that school can be a source of reparation by developing family counselling sessions, including rôle playing, socio-drama and psycho-drama. This type of support will help to counteract the loss of earlier developmental abilities.25 Whereas by 12 months a secure infant is in open two-way communication with his mother by means of gesture and expression that each partner interprets correctly, an insecure infant is not. Instead of turning to his mother to discover whether or not it is safe to approach and explore the toys and the visitor, and to indicate to her how he is feeling in the strange and changing situation, and how he would like her to respond, he tends to keep himself to himself, or else to give contradictory signals. Moreover, as the longitudinal studies conducted respectively by Mary Main in California and the Grossmanns in Bavaria show, these two-way patterns of communication tend to persist. Thus, at six years of age, mother-child pairs who were communicating freely and effectively at 12 months are continuing to do so, in contrast to the limited and inadequate communication patterns that are found to be still characteristic of pairs in which the child was earlier assessed as insecurely attached. 26

Parenting Issues Non-organic (endogenous) learning and behavioural difficulties have their origins in environmental consequences. Schubert, et al., have concluded that unwantedness by parents, especially the mother, may be a factor in increasing the likelihood of learning and behavioural difficulties as well as other social problems. Psychiatric abnormality was also found to have an increased occurrence in children whose sibling was less than two years older.27 Wanted children quite clearly seem to stand a better chance of surviving unscathed during the process of development. This chance improves even more if parents too are healthy, and if their developmental process was satisfactory and produced a happy childhood. However those who are at risk are not lost, and the purposes of the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development [USA] are designed in part to promote the interests of such children. Alexander gives some indication of the breadth of work accomplished by this comparatively new institute. It includes the promoting of the birth of healthy babies,

6

trying to assure that babies born are wanted, and that all children reach adulthood free of illness and disability with the possibility of achieving their potential.28 Mother-infant interactions are therefore important to the success of child-rearing. Schaefer observed 321 low-income mothers whilst they were interacting with their babies at home when the child was four months old and again at twelve months. These were then later related to teacher observations when the child was in kindergarten. Through factor analysis the dimension of positive interaction and gentle-sensitive against punitive-irritable were identified at the earlier assessments. Positive interaction was significantly correlated with child academic competence, but the less reliable and less stable dimension of gentlesensitive was not significantly correlated with child behaviour during kindergarten.29 A cultural change of recent times has been the large number of children being raised by a single parent. This has reached the political agenda in the UK as some views have been expressed that this situation may at times be entered into deliberately. A government report into ethnic minorities states that 54 per cent of young Afro-Caribbeans have no father at home. This compares with 33 per cent of Africans, 16 per cent of whites, 11 per cent of Chinese, and less than 10 per cent of Pakistanis, Bangladeshis and Indians. The report suggests that the rejection of marriage by West Indians reflects the culture of their countries of origin.30 One of the psychological issues here is the potential orchestration of gender by (a) parent(s). Bem who considers herself to be an empirical scientist has written extensively on this subject. Indeed she has both supported and negated the prospect of androgyny being a solution in child-rearing.31 Feminists have put forward proposals for what they see as solutions to sex inequality, but Bem finds that these are less than adequate. She says that she has never been persuaded that sex equality means an identical profile of characteristics between men and women.32 As a mother herself, Bem considers that sex-rôle stereotyping has a debilitating effect and she seeks to: free the human personality from the restricting prison of sex-rôles stereotyping and to develop a conception of mental health which is free from culturally imposed definitions of masculinity and femininity.

In this respect she lays down her conditions for such a state. These are: SEXUAL PREFERENCE should be considered to be consistent with any concept of mental health or ideal personality.

Androgyny The use of this description in the childrearing context implies confirmation of gender in respect of primary sexual characteristics, but making no distinctions in the usual areas of dress, appearance, activities and employment.

SEX-RÔLE IDENTITY. It should be practical and possible for people to exhibit naturally, both masculine and feminine characteristics of personality, but because men and women have been locked into their respective sex-rôles, women have been afraid to express preferences or anger, to trust their own judgement and to take control of situations. Men have been afraid to cry, to touch one another, or to own up to fears or weaknesses. An adult should be able to relate to other human beings as people. Limiting their response is unnecessarily destructive of human potential. The traditional situation has produced for women, dependency and self-denial; and for men, arrogance and exploitation, untempered by a sufficient concern for others. Bem’s prescription for a liberated sexual identity is stated thus: •

Let sexual preferences be ignored;



Let sex-rôles be abolished; and



Let gender move from figure to ground.

Bem sees a problem here of course and that is that the concept of androgyny could be seen as replacing masculine or feminine with masculine and feminine. This would load them with two potential sources of inadequacy. She goes on to point out that the concept of androgyny is a dangerous one from the perspective of gender-schema theory because

7

it is based on the presumption that there is a masculine and feminine in everyone, such that femininity and masculinity are an independent and palpable reality and are not just cognitive constructs derived from gender-schematic processing. Human behaviours and personality attributes should no longer be linked with gender, and society should stop projecting gender into situations irrelevant to genital characteristics. Turning to her alternative strategy she discusses the three main theories through which concepts of gender and sexuality have developed. PSYCHOANALYTIC THEORY was organised by Freud and places the child’s identification with the same sex parent as the primary mechanism whereby children become sex-typed, an identification that results from the child’s discovery of genital sex differences. SOCIAL LEARNING THEORY emphasises the rewards and punishments that children receive for sex-appropriate and sex-inappropriate behaviour. COGNITIVE-DEVELOPMENT THEORY focuses almost exclusively on the child as the primary agent of his or her own sex-rôle socialization. It assumes that sex-typing follows naturally and inevitably from universal principles of cognitive development. As an alternative to these, Bem proposes a Gender-Schema Theory. This enables sex-typing to be mediated by the child’s own cognitive processing, but deriving it from the sexdifferentiated practices of the social community. This establishes it as a learned phenomenon and as such it will be neither inevitable nor immutable. In considering how to raise gender-aschematic children, Bem sees the major option as trying to undermine the dominant ideology before it can itself do the undermining. She says that feminist parents cannot just ignore gender in their child-rearing as they might prefer to do, because society will then have a free rein to teach its own lessons on gender as it does with most other children. She sees instead what she calls a process of inoculating their children against gender-schematic processing. This comprises: •

enabling children to learn about sex differences, initially without also learning the culture’s sex-linked associative network by simultaneously retarding their children’s knowledge of the cultural correlates of sex and advancing their children’s knowledge of the biological correlates of sex;



parents providing alternative schemata that their children can use to interpret the culture’s sex-linked associative network when they do learn it. This avoids the pitfall of just learning gender-schematic processing somewhat later than their more traditional peers.

What then remains is just the biological correlates of sex; anatomy and reproduction. These are then known as the only definitive attributes of females and males. Bem sees all this as being on a theoretical basis only at the present time. She would like to see children raised with neither a gender-schema nor a sexism-schema. She regretfully submits that the present alternative is between a child becoming gender-schematic (sex-typed) or sexismschematic (feminist). Of these she prefers the latter. The question that might be asked here is: Does Bem’s theory interfere with adequate mothering? Rossi has suggested that there is a biologically based potential for a greater investment in a child by its mother than by its father, at least during its early months of life. She also questions whether fathers will participate in child-care on an equal basis if the are affected by their own negative developmental experiences in this area.33,34,35 Others have determined that mothering is identified as a female rôle by reason of training and identification. From this theory it is deduced that girls learn to be prepared for motherhood just as they learn other sex-appropriate behaviour. Chodorow declares that the process of child-care as practised by mothers is responsible for daughters who wish to become mothers themselves. Thus girls emerge from childhood with a capacity for the empathy required, already built into their self-image in a way that boys do not.36 Her view is that because mothers are the same gender as their daughters and have themselves been girls, mothers of daughters tend to experience their infant daughters as less separate from themselves than they do their infant sons.37

8

As a consequence of this finding, Rossi suggests that this pattern of early socialisation is a less sharply differentiated self in girls than in boys, and so in women there is a greater sensitivity to others, greater capacity for empathy, and a greater field dependency than in men. (Idem, 1987) Chodorow’s view has been criticised as being a retrospective analysis made from patients and therefore is not representative. Another theory is that the salary structure of past years has made it economically sound for the male to be employed and the female to take care of the children. An early but nevertheless telling comment was made by Balint when she wrote: … it remains self-evident that the interests of mother and child are identical, and it is the generally acknowledged measure of the goodness or badness of the mother how far she really feels this identity of interests.38

Chodorow challenges this concept stating that it will persist as long as (only) women mother, and that it is one major reason why equal parenting is a necessary component of sexual equality.39 She places the origins of the differentiation of the sexes into the realms of object relations, suggesting that it occurs during the emergence of separateness. She suggests that: …our own sense of differentiation, of separateness from others, as well as our psychological and cultural experience and interpretation of gender or sexual difference, are created through psychological, social, and cultural processes, and through relational experiences. We can only understand gender difference, and human distinctness and separation, relationally and situationally, as part of a system of asymmetrical social relationships embedded in inequalities of power, in which we grow up as selves, and as women and men. Our experience and perception of gender are processual; they are produced developmentally and in our daily social and cultural lives. (Idem, 1987)

Rossi takes Chodorow to task because she believes psychoanalytic theory is not the best vehicle for understanding normal behaviour. Chodorow states that the strength of psychoanalysis is as an interpretative theory and not as a behavioural science. Rossi counters this when she writes: “I cannot accept… the view that, ‘Pathology reflects in exaggerated form, differences in what are in fact normal tendencies’ ”. Rossi adds that pathology can involve not just exaggerated normal tendencies but actual reversals from normal tendencies. She also makes some telling comments about her terminology, stating that, “Only if one has learned to translate her terminology can a reader understand her meaning …”. Thus she finds Chodorow’s use of clinical cases, without the comparison to controls, not to be a suitable design to enable an understanding of normal developmental processes.

1

2

3

4

5

6

7

Irwin, O. C. Infant speech: the effect of family occupational status and of age on use of sound types. J. Speech Hearing Dis. 13: [pp., 224-226]; 1948. Sears, R. R., Maccoby, E. E., and Levin, H. Patterns of Child Rearing. New York, USA, Harper & Row, 1957. Catalano, F. L., & McCarthy, D. Infant speech as a possible predictor of later intelligence. J. Psychol., 38: [pp., 203-209]; 1954. Pathak, M.P.; Saxena, N.K.; & Mishra, L.S. Speech development in mentally retarded children and some specific psychosomatic factors. Indian Psycholog. Review; 25(1/4); Special Issue. [Pp., 31-37]; 1983. Skuse, D. Extreme deprivation in early childhood: A reply. Jnl. Child Psychol. & Psychiat. & Allied Discp. 26(5); [pp., 827-828]; 1985. Cooper, R.P.; & Aslin, R.N. The language environment of the young infant: Implications for early perceptual development. Special Issue: Infant perceptual development. Canadian Jnl. Psychol.; 43(2); [pp., 247-265]; 1989. Milner, E. A study of the relationship between reading readiness in grade one school children and patterns of parent-child interaction. Child. Develop., 22: [pp., 95-112]; 1951.

9

8

9

10

11

12

13

14

15 16

17

18 19

20

21

22

23

24

25

26

27

28

29

30 31

DeBaryshe, B.D.; & Binder, J.C. Development of an instrument for measuring parental beliefs about reading aloud to young children. Perceptual & Motor Skills; 78(3/2) Special Issue; [pp., 1303-1311]; 1994. Toomey, D.; & Sloane, J. Fostering children’s early literacy development through parent involvement: A five-year programme. In Dickinson, D.K. [Ed.] Bridges to Literacy: Children, families and schools. Cambridge, MA, USA; Blackwell; 1994. Bus, A.G.; & Van Ijzendoorn, M.H. Patterns of attachment in frequently and infrequently reading mother-child dyads. Jnl. Gen. Psychol. 153(4); [pp., 395-403]; 1992. Scarborough, H.S.; Dobrich, W.; & Hager, M. Pre-school literacy experience and later reading achievement. Jnl. Learng. Disab. 24(8); [pp., 508-511]; 1991. Deutsch, M. The rôle of social class in language development and cognition. Amer.J. Orthopsychiat., 35, [pp., 78-88.]; 1965. Bernstein, B. Some sociological determinants of perception: an inquiry into subcultural differences. Brit. J. Sociol., 9: [pp., 159-174]; 1958. Bernstein, B. A public language: some socio-logical implications of a linguistic form. Ibid., 10: [pp., 311-326. 1959. Bernstein, B. Language and social class. Ibid., 11:[pp., 271-276]; 1960. Pringle, M. L. K., & Bossio, V. A study of deprived children. Part I. Intellectual, emotional and social development. Part II. Language development and reading attainments. Vita Humana, 1: [pp., 142-170]; 1958. Pringle, M. L. K., & Tanner, M. The effects of early deprivation on speech development: a comparative study of four year olds in a nursery school and in residential nurseries. Language and Speech, 1: [pp., 269-287]; 1958. Anastasi, Anne. Differential Psychology (3rd ed.). New York: Macmillan, 1958. Arsenian, S. Bilingualism in the post-war world, Psychological Bulletin, 1945, 42, [pp., 65-86]. Darcy, Natalie T. A review of the literature on the effects of bilingualism upon the measurement of intelligence. Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1953, 82, [pp., 21-57]. Anastasi, Anne, & Cordova, F. A. Some effects of bilingualism upon the intelligence test performance of Puerto Rican children in New York City. Journal of Educational Psychology, 1953, 44, [pp., 1-19]. Spoerl, Dorothy T. Bilinguality and emotional adjustment. Journal of Abnormal and Social Psychology, 1943, 38, [pp., 37-57]. Willig, A.C. A case of blaming the victim: The Dunn monograph on bilingual Hispanic children on the US mainland. Special Issue: Achievement testing: Science vs. ideology. Hisp. Jnl. Behav. Sci. ; 10(3); [pp., 219-236]; 1989. Chall, J.S. From Language to Reading and Reading to Language. ASHA Reports Series, 17; [pp., 28-33] Washington DC, USA.1989. Mohan, V. Counselling of socially disadvantaged child. Asian Jnl. Of Psychol. & Educn. 11(2); [pp., 1-8]; 1988. Bowlby, J. Postscript. In Parkes, C.M.; Stevenson-Hinde, J. & Marris, P. [Eds.]. Attachments Across the Life Cycle. Tavistock/Routledge, London; 1991. Schubert, H.J.; Wagner, M.E.: & Schubert, D.S. Child spacing effects: A comparison of institutionalised and normal children. Jnl. Devlop. & Behav. Ped. 4(4); [pp., 262-264]; 1983. Alexander, D.F. Prevention of disabilities: Priorities and research directions. In Thompson, T; & Hupp, S.C. [Eds.] Saving Children at Risk: Poverty and disabilities. Newbury Park, CA, USA; Sage, 1992. Schaefer, E.S. Dimensions of mother-infant interaction: Measurement, stability and predictive validity. Inf. Behav. & Develop., 12(4); [pp., 379-393]; 1989. Report in The Times, 8.8.1996 Bem, S.L. Probing the Promise of Androgyny. [Pp., 206-225] and Gender Schema Theory and its Implications for Child Development: Raising Gender-aschematic Children in a Gender-schematic Society. In Walsh, M.R. [Ed.]; The Psychology of Women. New Haven, USA & London UK, Yale Univ. Press, 1987.

10

32

33

34 35

36

37

38

39

Rossi, A S. On The Reproduction of Mothering: A methodological debate. In Walsh, M.R. [Ed.]; The Psychology of Women. New Haven, USA & London UK, Yale Univ. Press, 1987. Rossi, A.S. A biosocial perspective on parenting. Daedalus, 106 (2), [pp., 1-31]; 1977. Rossi, A S. The biosocial side of parenting. Human Nature, 1 (6); [pp., 72-79]; 1978. Rossi, A S. Gender and parenthood. American Sociological Association, 1983 presidential address. American Sociological Review, 49 (2), [pp., 1-l9]; 1984. Chodorow, N. The Reproduction of Mothering: Psychoanalysis and the sociology of gender. Berkeley, USA, University of California Press. 1978. Chodorow, N. Family structure and feminine personality. In Rosaldo, M.Z.; & Lamphere, L. [Eds.] Women, Culture and Society. Stanford, Calif. USA, Stanford Univ. Press, 1974. Balint, A. Love for the Mother and Mother Love. (1939) In Balint, M. Primary Love and Psycho-analytic Technique. [pp., 97]; New York, USA, Liveright, 1965. Chodorow, N. Feminism and Difference: Gender, Relation, and Difference in Psychoanalytic Perspective. In Walsh, M.R. [Ed.]; The Psychology of Women. New Haven, USA & London UK, Yale Univ. Press, 1987.

11

Related Documents


More Documents from ""