Benoit Bird 2004

  • November 2019
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Benoit Bird 2004 as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 8,184
  • Pages: 10
Marine Biology (2004) 145: 435–444 DOI 10.1007/s00227-004-1339-1

R ES E AR C H A RT I C L E

K. J. Benoit-Bird

Prey caloric value and predator energy needs: foraging predictions for wild spinner dolphins

Received: 17 April 2003 / Accepted: 16 February 2004 / Published online: 25 March 2004 Ó Springer-Verlag 2004

Abstract Spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) feed on individual small (2–10 cm long) prey that undergo diel vertical migrations, presumably making them inaccessible to dolphins during the day. To examine how time, prey behavior, prey distribution, and energy needs constrain dolphin foraging, a calorimeter was used to measure the caloric content of prey items. These data were combined with information on prey distribution in the field and the energetic needs of dolphins to construct basic bioenergetic models predicting the total prey consumption and mean feeding rates of wild dolphins as well as potential prey preferences. The mean caloric density of mesopelagic animals from Hawaii was high (2,837 cal/g wet weight for shrimps, squids, and myctophid fishes). Their total caloric content, however, was low because of their small size. Energy value of prey and energetic needs of spinner dolphins were used to examine the effect of time and energy constraints on dolphin foraging. The results predict that spinner dolphins need to consume an estimated minimum of 1.25 large prey items per minute to meet their maintenance energy needs. If the additional energy costs of foraging are considered, the estimated necessary foraging rate is predicted to increase only slightly when large prey are consumed. If smaller prey are consumed, the total energy demand may be twice the basic maintenance value. Prey density and size are predicted to be important in determining if dolphins can forage successfully, meeting their energetic needs. The prey size predictions compare well with results from previous gut content studies and from stomach contents of a recently stranded spinner dolphin that had enough prey in its stomach to meet its estimated basic maintenance energy needs for a day. Communicated by P.W. Sammarco, Chauvin K. J. Benoit-Bird Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology, P.O. Box 1106, Kailua, HI 96734, USA E-mail: [email protected] Tel.: +1-808-2475063 Fax: +1-808-2475831

Finally, the results suggest that spinner dolphins are time and therefore efficiency limited rather than being limited by the total amount of available prey. This may explain the diel migration exhibited by spinner dolphins that allows them to follow the movements of their prey and presumably maximizes their foraging time.

Introduction Bioenergetics models of foraging have provided useful predictions about the behavior and population dynamics of many predators. For example, models incorporating prey energy values and predator energetic needs have been used to predict dive times in blue whales (AcevedoGutierrez et al. 2002), total hunting time for wild dogs (Gorman et al. 1998), and growth rates in flounder (Burke and Rice 2002). Models incorporating foraging energetics in pelagic predators have also been useful in examining prey preferences and understanding the impact of the predator on the ecosystem (Bunce 2001). These kinds of predictions are particularly useful when studying populations that are difficult to observe directly, such as pelagic marine animals. These models provide testable hypotheses for data that are often more easily measured than the parameters of the model and can also provide explanations for observed behaviors. Spinner dolphins, Stenella longirostris, a subtropical pelagic species, consume small, mesopelagic prey, presumably one individual at a time (Norris and Dohl 1980). Analyses of stomach contents of spinner dolphins revealed that they are primarily nocturnal feeders; their stomachs are full when caught early in the day and empty when caught at other times of the day (Norris and Dohl 1980). Stomachs of spinners caught off the coast of West Africa (Cadenat and Doutre 1959) and in the eastern tropical Pacific (Perrin et al. 1973) contained mostly myctophid fish. Fitch and Brownell (1968) found that two species of myctophid fishes, Benthosema panamense and Lampanyctus parvicauda, accounted for more

436

than 50% of the spinner’s diet in the eastern tropical Pacific but animals from this area also had large numbers of individuals from other mesopelagic fish families including Gonostomatidae, Melamphaeidae, and Bregmacerotidae (Fitch and Brownell 1968; Perrin et al. 1973). Mesopelagic squid species, mostly from the family Enoploteuthidae, also accounted for a large proportion (nearly 30%) of the prey consumed by spinner dolphins in the eastern tropical Pacific (Perrin et al. 1973). Stomach contents of spinner dolphins caught or stranded in Hawaiian waters are similar to those caught in other locations, containing primarily small (2–10 cm) mesopelagic fish and squid. Norris et al. (1994) found that more than 50% by number of the contents of Hawaiian spinner dolphin stomachs were myctophid fish. Hawaiian spinner dolphins also consumed large proportions of mesopelagic squid; Abralia trigonura and A. astrosticta were the most abundant species (Norris and Dohl 1980; Clarke and Young 1998). One difference in the diet of Hawaiian spinner dolphins from that of animals from other locations is the relatively large contribution of the sergestid crustacean Sergia lucens to their diet, as well as a small contribution by pasiphaeid crustaceans (Norris and Dohl 1980; Norris et al. 1994). Nearly all of the species identified from the stomachs of Hawaiian spinner dolphins are components of the mesopelagic boundary community (Reid et al. 1991). The mesopelagic boundary community is a land-associated community that has a composition of fish, shrimp, and squid that is distinct from the micronektonic community found in waters beyond the archipelago’s slopes (Reid 1994). Many of the species found in the boundary community migrate diurnally, reaching depths of 400–700 m during the day and ranging from the surface to 400 m at night (Reid 1994). The boundary community also migrates horizontally, moving nearly 2 km toward shore until midnight, when it achieves its maximum density in waters approximately 1 km from the shoreline. After midnight, the community then migrates away from shore as it descends (Benoit-Bird et al. 2001). Rather than foraging offshore for the entire night, spinner dolphins track the horizontal migration of their prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). It is hypothesized that this tracking of their prey allows spinner dolphins to maximize their foraging time while foraging on the prey at its highest densities (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). To examine the effect of time and energy constraints on spinner dolphins, data on the energy content of spinner dolphin prey and estimates of the foraging needs of spinner dolphins are presented. Prey energy density values are crucial inputs to bioenergetic consumption models but were not available for mesopelagic micronekton in general, or for the specific mesopelagic prey of spinner dolphins in Hawaii. The effects of various foraging costs on energy needs and foraging rates, as well as the relationship between available prey and spinner dolphin energy needs, were examined to determine what factors may be important in driving the diel migration

pattern observed in spinner dolphins. This approach is conceptually similar to the bioenergetics model of Stockwell and Johnson (1999), which incorporated foraging constraints to predict diel migration observed in fish. Simple models were also used to predict potential prey preferences, both by size and by taxonomic identity. While limited information is available for estimating the parameters of the model, the relative predictions of the model are robust to changes in these parameters and provide information on the importance of prey size for potential foraging success. Model predictions were preliminarily compared with results from the stomach contents of a single stranded animal as well as previously published gut contents studies.

Materials and methods Prey value Animals from the mesopelagic boundary community were captured alive using an Isaacs–Kidd midwater trawl. Their backscattering strength (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001), wet weight, dry weight, displacement volume, and caloric content (Benoit-Bird and Au 2002) were measured. Calorie measurements were made using a bomb calorimeter.

Total caloric needs No food intake values are available for spinner dolphins, Stenella longirostris, either in the wild or in captivity. To estimate the total energy required for maintenance of a spinner dolphin, the kilocalories per kilogram of body weight required to maintain a captive striped dolphin, Stenella coeruleoalba (Kastelein et al. 2002a), was applied to the full range of spinner dolphin weights and compared with the average maintenance energy values for other delphinids in captivity.

Stomach contents of a stranded spinner dolphin As a preliminary test of the viability of the model proposed, the stomach contents of a stranded animal were examined and compared, along with published reports of spinner dolphin gut contents, with the model predictions. A sub-adult female spinner dolphin, weighing approximately 45 kg, stranded early in the morning of 7 July 2002 on Magic Island, Ala Moana Park on the south shore of Oahu. Despite attempts to save and rehabilitate the animal, it died. The animal appeared well nourished and no cause of death was apparent after necropsy. The stomach of the animal was dissected within 24 h of death, and the identifiable prey parts, including fish otoliths, squid lenses and beaks, and whole animals, were frozen, measured, and identified to the lowest taxonomic level possible. To determine a lower limit to the caloric content of the prey consumed by this animal, the caloric content was estimated from the wet weight of the stomach contents. Unfortunately, no catalog exists for the otoliths of the myctophids or the beaks of the squids found in the Hawaiian boundary layer so the caloric content of individual prey items could not be calculated by relating the size of the hard parts to the length of the prey items. The distribution of prey caloric content per gram of wet weight was unimodal and not significantly different from a normal distribution. Because there were no significant differences between groups, the mean calories per gram of wet weight for all groups was multiplied by the total weight of the stomach contents to estimate the total caloric value of the consumed prey.

437 The data available on animals from the Hawaiian boundary community is limited, making it impossible to estimate the length of prey from the hard parts in the stomach. Two methods were used to estimate the average length of prey items eaten. First, the total calorie–length relationship for squid and myctophids was applied to the estimated average caloric content of each prey item. Second, we applied the wet weight–length relationship for squid and myctophids separately to the average wet weight of each prey item. The total length of any whole animals was directly measured. Models and parameters Simple models based on the energetic needs of dolphins and the energy content of prey were used to predict the number of prey that an individual spinner dolphin needs to consume. Information on prey behavior was used to convert these estimates to average rates of prey consumption. The consequences of foraging costs, derived from the literature, on these estimates were explored. The effects of prey size on rate estimates were also examined. The effects of prey size on foraging estimates allowed prediction of prey size preference. Possible preferences for prey type were investigated based on the acoustic scattering of prey and the swimming ability of prey obtained from the literature. Finally, information on the distribution of prey in the field was combined with the foraging models to understand the limitations faced by spinner dolphins in the wild.

fit (R2=0.76) after removing the single outlier, a nearly 10-cm-long myctophid. The relationship between the wet weight of individual animals and their energy content in calories is shown in Fig. 1b (calories=2,496*wet weight+55; R2=0.95). The outlier did not significantly affect this relationship. The distribution of prey caloric content per gram of wet weight (Fig. 2) was unimodal and approximated a normal distribution. There were also no significant differences in the caloric content per gram of animal wet weight between shrimp, myctophids, and squid (ANOVA; df=2, P>0.10). The caloric density of animals from the mesopelagic boundary community is high (mean 2,837 cal/g wet weight) relative to food commonly fed to marine mammals in captivity (Table 1). Only anchovies and sardines, both high in oil content like mesopelagic animals, come close to the energy density of the food consumed by spinner dolphins in the wild (Kizevetter 1971/1973; Honda et al. 1992). Total caloric needs

Results Prey value The total caloric content of individual prey items from the boundary community measured with a bomb calorimeter increases with increasing animal length (Fig. 1a). The relationship can be described by a power function for myctophid fishes, and a linear function approximately describes the relationship for shrimp and squid where E is energy content in calories, and L is total length in centimeters for all three groups. EFish ¼ 8.8  L2:64 ; R2 ¼ 0.87; n ¼ 58

The maintenance requirement for a striped dolphin, 54 kcal/kg of dolphin per day (Kastelein et al. 2002a), compares well with the average maintenance energy values of 50 kcal/kg of dolphin per day reported for other delphinids in captivity (J. Pawloski and M. Breese, personal communication). The estimated kilocalories, assuming a 54 kcal/kg maintenance need, required for maintenance each day by spinner dolphins of various sizes are shown in Table 2. These represent the best estimates available for the average maintenance energy needs for a spinner dolphin. Differences in these values affect the results of the model by shifting curves but do not change the relative relationships, so for simplicity, these fixed values are utilized throughout.

EShrimp ¼ 228  L  78.2, R2 ¼ 0.87; n ¼ 14 ESquid ¼ 214  L  379.6; R2 ¼ 0.80; n ¼ 8

Stomach contents of a stranded spinner dolphin

A single linear relationship can be fitted through all three groups pooled for simplification of analysis (ETotal=388*L)1,139) while retaining a relatively strong

The entire contents of the stomach weighed 834 g (wet) and had a displacement volume of 3.7 l. The identified contents included 2 intact shrimp (both Sergia fulgens),

Fig. 1 a Prey total caloric content, measured with a bomb calorimeter, of mesopelagic animals from the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary layer as a function of animal length. b Total caloric value of all three taxonomic groups of mesopelagic prey from the Hawaiian boundary layer was strongly correlated with wet weight

438

estimate of the average squid total length of 11.4 cm and an average myctophid total length of 7.8 cm. Applying the wet weight–length relationship for squid and myctophids separately to the average wet weight of each prey item yielded an estimate for the average squid total length of 8.4 cm and an average myctophid total length of 7.7 cm. The total length of the 117 whole squids extracted from the stomach ranged from 2.4 to 9.6 cm, with a mean of 7.7 cm (Fig. 3).

Models and parameters

Fig. 2 Distribution of calories per gram of wet weight for all mesopelagic boundary community animals measured. No significant differences were observed between myctophid fishes, shrimps, and squids

1,012 fish otoliths (96% were identified as myctophids: 88% Benthosema, 8% Diaphus), 117 whole squids [all identified as mesopelagic boundary community species (Reid et al. 1991)], 720 squid eye lenses, and 102 squid beaks. These prey bodies and parts were interpreted to represent at least 985 individual prey. Because spinner dolphins captured in the afternoon typically have completely empty stomachs (Perrin et al. 1973, 1999), it is likely that all 985 of the prey items represented in the stomach contents were eaten the night before the animal stranded. The estimated caloric content of the stomach contents was about 2,400 kcal, making the average caloric content of each prey item about 2,400 cal. Because of the degree of digestion and consideration of only the stomach, not the entire gut, this should be regarded as a minimum estimate of the calories consumed by this animal. The maintenance energy needs of a spinner dolphin this size, without considering possible growth costs, were estimated at 2,440 kcal per day (Table 2). The size of prey consumed by this dolphin was of interest for preliminarily examining the results from the models presented. The total calorie–length relationship for squid and myctophids was applied to the estimated average caloric content of each prey item, giving an Table 1 Mean caloric densities (calories per gram of wet weight) for prey items commonly fed to marine mammals compared with prey of Hawaiian spinner dolphins and other midwater micronektonic animals

Boundary community Anchovy Sardines Capelin Herring Other midwater vertical migrators Mackeral Cod Smelt Squid

The steps in the derivation of the models and their parameters build successively; each component of the model is presented under a separate heading along with justification for parameter choices, a more detailed description of the methods used to generate the results, and the important findings. Foraging costs Maintenance energy requirements obtained from captive studies include only costs associated with feeding and routine activities, and not those associated with foraging. To understand the importance of food to spinner dolphin behavior, the effects of foraging costs on energy needs and food intake rates need to be explored. The energetic costs experienced by foraging spinner dolphins come from swimming, diving, acoustic searching, and ‘herding’ of prey and finally from behaviors associated with the capture of the prey. Spinner dolphins foraging on the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary community move at least 8 km back and forth over the slope of the island each night while following the horizontal migration of their prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). Near the beginning and end of the prey’s migration, spinner dolphins probably dive to depths of at least 150 m. Near the apex of the migration, their diving depths are reduced to an average of 25 m (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). Hawaiian spinner dolphins feed cooperatively, in pairs, within relatively large groups (20 or more animals). They appear to actively concentrate their prey by swimming in a circle about 25 m in diameter around the outside edges of the prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a).

Mean caloric density

Source

2,837 2,808 2,300 1,266–1,863 1,480–1,831 1,800

This article Kizevetter 1971/1973 Honda et al. 1992 Martensson et al. 1996 Kastelein et al. 2002b; Martensson et al. 1996 Brooks 1977

1,700 1,003)1,340 1,200 852

Cox et al. 1996 Smith et al. 1997; Martensson et al. 1996 M. Breese, personal communication Cox et al. 1996

439 Table 2 Estimated kilocalories required by spinner dolphins each day for maintenance

Stranded animal Small adult female Average adult Large adult male

Dolphin weight (kg)

kcal day)1

45 55 65 75

2,430 2,970 3,520 4,050

Fig. 4 The number of prey items necessary per day as a function of mean prey length eaten for an average-size spinner dolphin to meet its basic energy requirements at different fixed search costs per prey item

Fig. 3 Distribution of squid total length for animals extracted whole from the stomach of a stranded Hawaiian spinner dolphin

The foraging costs to spinner dolphins of finding prey, diving to the layer, and ‘herding’ are associated with prey patches and are relatively isolated from individual prey, so they will be referred to collectively as ‘search’ costs. The energetic costs associated with prey search (swimming, acoustic searching, diving, and ‘herding’) are not likely to be affected by the size of individual prey. The costs of swimming (see Yazdi et al. 1999) over the island’s slopes to find prey would not be dependent on prey size. Acoustic prey searching ability at a large scale is not affected by individual prey size, but by the distribution of the prey patch or entire scattering layer because of volume reverberation (Urick 1983). Costs associated with diving to the prey layer (see Williams et al. 1999) are probably independent of prey size because the depth of prey is not strongly correlated with individual animal size in the upper 150 m (Reid 1994). Finally, the conserved nature of the observed cooperative ‘herding’ behavior of prey by spinner dolphins (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a) suggests relatively constant effort is used to herd each prey patch. These search costs are amortized over all prey in each patch successfully captured in a foraging bout, and consequently, within a foraging bout they would be the same for each item, regardless of the item’s caloric content. Little information is available with which to estimate search costs for spinner dolphins directly; however, their effects on spinner dolphin total energetic needs and foraging rates can be investigated. A range of search

costs from 0.00 to 0.15 kcal per prey item was investigated. This represents an increase of 0–8% over the animal’s maintenance energy during active foraging. Increases in metabolic rate of 10% for efficient swimming (Yazdi et al. 1999) and 20% or more for diving have been observed in dolphins (Williams et al. 1999). Because dolphins may not be swimming and diving for the entire foraging time, lower costs were used in the calculations. However, values up to a 50% increase over maintenance energy do not affect the conclusions of the analysis. The effect of adding fixed costs on the number of prey that must be consumed by an average-size spinner dolphin to meet its maintenance energy costs, calculated using the estimated net gain of energy for individual prey of each size, is shown in Fig. 4. In all cases, the number of prey necessary to meet the animal’s energy needs decreases with increasing prey size. When no search cost is assessed and prey average 2.5 cm in length, a total of nearly 15,000 prey items must be eaten. When a dolphin consumes large prey, averaging 10 cm in length (the largest prey available within the boundary layer), about 825 prey items must be consumed. If search costs remain the same regardless of prey size, the costs will have a greater impact on an animal foraging on small prey. When a meager 0.15 kcal/prey cost is added (less than the increase in metabolic costs of swimming for the entire foraging bout), the estimated number of prey necessary to meet a dolphin’s predicted energy needs goes up to nearly 40,000 for a 2.5-cm prey item (Figure 4). This is a more than threefold increase over the estimate with no search cost. For 10-cm prey, the estimated increase in the number of prey that must be consumed when a 0.15 kcal/prey cost is added only represents a few percent over the number that must be

440

consumed when no cost is assessed. The results can also be looked at as the estimated total calories that must be consumed by an individual dolphin as a function of prey size at different search costs (Fig. 5a). This suggests that animals should focus foraging effort on prey items about 6 cm in total length or longer to minimize the number of prey and consequently the total number of calories they need to consume. Changing the maintenance energy of the individual or changing the search costs to higher values has no effect on this prediction. Search costs are probably independent of prey size, but prey capture costs probably increase with prey size because prey mobility also increases. If prey capture costs increase proportionately with prey size as observed in fish (Sambilay 1990) and squid (Yatsu et al. 1999), the simplest case, then the prey capture cost can be expressed as a percentage of the post-search value of the prey (net prey value=gross prey caloric content)0.0– 0.15 kcal fixed search cost per prey)capture cost expressed as a percentage caloric content of each prey; Fig. 5b). The values selected are within the same range as the percentage increase in metabolic rates associated with burst swimming over efficient swimming (5–20%; Yazdi et al. 1999). This added prey capture cost decreases the slope of the total calorie curve between small and large prey items. Changing the prey capture cost to a higher value shifts the curve up and further decreases the difference in the estimated total calories that must be consumed between small and large prey. However, even at the lowest fixed search cost (0.05 kcal/prey), it is not possible to negate the differences between small and large prey in the estimated total calories that need to be consumed by the dolphin. Regardless of the prey capture cost, dolphins are still predicted to preferentially consume larger prey.

Fig. 5 a Total calories necessary per day for an average-size spinner dolphin to meet its basic energy requirements as a function of prey size at different fixed per-prey-item search costs. b Total calories that must be consumed by an average-size spinner dolphin to meet its basic energy requirements with both a fixed search cost per prey item and a prey capture cost that increases with prey size

Other costs Relatively little information is available on the energetic costs of lactation or pregnancy in cetaceans. Captive bottlenose dolphins did not change the number of calories consumed during gestation but increased their caloric consumption by 52–97% during lactation (Kastelein et al. 2002b). Other mammals that feed their offspring for a similar amount of time, including hamsters and humans, also increase their energy consumption by more than 50% during lactation (Day et al. 2002; Dufour et al. 2002). Estimated increases in the average-size female spinner dolphin’s food intake by various percentages to represent the energetic costs of lactation over maintenance energy are shown in Fig. 6. The data in this figure can also be used to estimate changes in food intake due to other causes such as growth, health, and environmental changes, among others, as these change the maintenance energy requirements. For example, growth in a subadult captive dusky dolphin resulted in approximately a 10% increase in maintenance energy over the 50 kcal/kg base value (Kastelein et al. 2000), resulting in a modest increase in estimated total energy that must be consumed by this growing animal. How fast must a dolphin eat? Spinner dolphins have a limited effective foraging depth, estimated at between 200 and 250 m (Fitch and

Fig. 6 Estimated total caloric needs for the average-size female spinner dolphin at different non-foraging costs. The costs of foraging on 7.5-cm-long prey are included at both a fixed, per prey item level, and a fixed plus percentage foraging cost. The dotted region shows the range of costs of lactation observed in captive bottlenose dolphins by Kastelein et al. (2002b). This graph can also be used to examine the effects of other costs such as growth or stress. Growth, for example, added a 10% cost to maintenance needs for a captive dolphin of another species (Kastelein et al. 2000)

441

Brownell 1968; Dolar et al. 2003) although they may be able to dive to depths of up to 400 m (Dolar et al. 2003). This means that the boundary community is inaccessible to spinner dolphins during the day due to its depth at between 400 and 700 m at that time. Based on the vertical migration patterns observed in the boundary layer by Reid (1994) and Benoit-Bird et al. (2001), the boundary layer is probably accessible to spinner dolphins for approximately 11 h of each day. An averagesize adult spinner dolphin eating only the largest prey available (10 cm in length) would need to average an estimated 1.25 prey items per minute of its probable foraging time for maintenance energy. A dolphin consuming 2.5 cm prey is predicted to average 22 prey/min over the 11 h the boundary community is available to spinner dolphins if no foraging costs are considered. Adding both prey search and capture costs does not substantially alter the predicted rate of a dolphin consuming large prey, increasing it to only an estimated maximum of 1.4 prey/min. If the length of prey averaged 7.5 cm, the model estimates that between 3 and 4 prey/ min would need to be consumed, depending on search and capture costs. If prey were on average only 2.5 cm long, the estimated number of prey consumed per minute would range between 22 and 46, depending on total foraging costs.

Possible prey preferences No strong differences in the amount of energy available within the different major taxonomic groups of mesopelagic prey around Hawaii are apparent, so no advantage of prey groups is clearly evident (no proximate analyses were conducted so no conclusions can be made about other potential differences in food quality). However, there are probably differences in the costs of foraging upon the different prey groups, particularly in prey detection and handling. Assuming spinner dolphins use echolocation to find their prey, the backscattering strength of prey items determines the ability of cetaceans to detect them. Shrimp and squid have a much smaller backscattering cross-section than myctophids, meaning that they are weaker echolocation targets (Fig. 7). Although spinner dolphins use a very different sonar signal than the one used by Benoit-Bird and Au (2001) to measure scattering from spinner dolphin prey, their relative scattering strengths probably remain similar. The difference in scattering strength between fish, shrimp, and squid has been observed at individual frequencies between 30 and 220 kHz (MacLennan and Simmonds 1992), covering the entire frequency range of spinner dolphin echolocation signals (Schotten et al. 2004). Because of these differences in scattering strength, individual shrimp and squid would be detectable to an echolocating dolphin over less range than myctophid fishes. There are also important differences in the ability of sonar to determine prey size between the prey groups, significant because of the hypothesized effect of prey size

Fig. 7 The relationship between total length of individual prey taxonomic groups and their dorsal-aspect acoustic backscattering cross-section at 200 kHz (Redrawn from Benoit-Bird and Au 2001)

on a spinner dolphin’s ability to meet its energetic needs. While all groups of prey exhibit an increase in backscattering strength with increasing length, the differences in backscatter from shrimp and squid over the range of available prey sizes is small (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001). Because backscattering strength is also affected by orientation of the animal (Benoit-Bird and Au 2001) and its behavior (Arnaya et al. 1989a, 1989b), these small differences probably make it difficult for a dolphin to estimate accurately the size of a shrimp or squid using echolocation. Myctophid fishes show a much stronger length–backscattering strength relationship, making it easier to assess the prey’s size with echolocation before it is consumed. If echolocation is important in prey detection and selection in spinner dolphins, myctophids should be preferred over shrimp or squid of equal size. Once prey is detected and selected, there are probably differences in the ability of spinner dolphins to capture prey of the different groups. Large differences between groups exist in the swimming rates of individuals of similar size. Sustained swimming speeds in a species of shrimp in the same genus, of the same size and body type, as the most abundant Hawaiian boundary community shrimp, reach 0.06 m/s, with burst speeds of 0.2 m/s (Cowles 2001). Similar burst speeds have been observed in krill, similar in size and body plan to mesopelagic boundary shrimp, when moving forward; however, reverse thrusts can reach 1.1 m/s and can cover 0.6 m (Kils 1979). Hawaiian boundary community myctophids reach much higher sustained speeds of 0.75 m/s (Reid 1994). Burst swimming speeds of fish with the size and fin type of myctophids are estimated at around 3 m/s (Sambilay 1990). Burst speeds of squid (corrected for body length) are even higher at 8 m/s, with cruising speeds of 0.8 m/s (Yatsu et al. 1999). Based on swimming speed estimates, shrimp are probably the easiest common prey for spinner dolphins to capture, with myctophids intermediate, and squid the most difficult.

442

The distribution of mesopelagic boundary community species and their relative abundance is currently unknown. However, the combination of ease of detection and size determination of myctophids with echolocation and their moderate swimming abilities compared with the other prey choices suggest that they should be preferentially consumed over the other two prey groups. Shrimp and squid would both be more difficult to detect; however the order of magnitude slower burst speeds of shrimp suggest that, given equal sizes, they should be preferentially consumed over squid. Information on the distribution of these groups will help to refine these predictions based on diving costs of dolphins and relative availability of the prey groups.

Because of the constancy of these differences, differences in the population sizes that could be supported by these prey layers are predicted. If total available prey energy is more important in determining spinner dolphin foraging success, the population density of spinner dolphins off Hawaii’s leeward coast is predicted to be roughly double that of the leeward Oahu population. If prey density is more important for foraging success, the density of spinner dolphins off the leeward Oahu coast should be higher. Information on the population densities of spinner dolphins is not available, but differences in population sizes between locations may provide insights into the foraging mechanisms used by spinner dolphins. Preliminary model assessment

Total prey availability The density of mesopelagic boundary community animals around the Hawaiian Islands varies as a function of time, distance from the shoreline, and location. The mean density of animals found nearshore during the night is between 5 and 25 animals/m3. Maximum densities can reach up to 1,800 animals/m3 (Benoit-Bird et al. 2001). Areas with spinner dolphins have the highest densities observed (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). Utilizing the acoustic scattering–caloric content relationships established by Benoit-Bird and Au (2002) for mesopelagic boundary community animals, the minimum available energy within the layer was estimated. To simplify estimates, only the myctophid relationship was utilized, giving the most conservatively low estimate of energy possible. The mean caloric density of the mesopelagic boundary community is estimated at 83 kcal/m3 with a maximum of nearly 9,000 kcal/m3. The average caloric density of areas in which spinner dolphins were found ranged between 112 and 186 kcal/m3, with each single, discrete patch of prey containing spinner dolphins having between 1.65*106 kcal and 5.95*106 kcal (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). These patches in which spinner dolphins were foraging have enough energy to support the estimated maintenance energy needs of between 510 and 1,830 average-size spinner dolphins if they could exploit all the prey within the patch. The maximum number of dolphins observed in a single patch at any one time was 26, much lower than could be supported by the patch. Large, consistent differences in the numerical density and available energy of prey were observed between coastlines (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003b). The density of animals in the nearshore layer off the leeward coast of Oahu during the night averaged 1.6 times the density observed off the leeward coast of Hawaii near Kealakekua Bay (Benoit-Bird et al. 2001). However, the total available energy per kilometer surveyed off Hawaii averaged twice that off the leeward coast of Oahu because of differences in the vertical extent of the layer. While prey off Hawaii were found in discrete, relatively small patches (tens of meters across), the prey off Oahu were found in extensive layers, covering kilometers.

Stomach contents from individual dolphins probably vary widely, particularly when considering only stranded animals. The estimate of the caloric value of prey in the stomach of this animal cannot be considered a perfect measure of the energy consumed, particularly as the contents of the remainder of the digestive track were not saved, but it does represent the minimum this animal consumed on this night. The stomach contents of this animal show that it is possible for a wild dolphin to meet the minimum energy needs proposed, consume the large number of prey predicted by the model in the time it had to forage, and to fit the volume of this prey inside its stomach.

Discussion The energy available within the mesopelagic boundary community far exceeds the energy necessary to support the spinner dolphin population off the leeward coasts of Oahu and Hawaii where spinner dolphins are most commonly sighted, and it is very densely distributed (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003b). Each discrete patch in which dolphins were found but in which no other boundary community predators were detected had enough prey to support an order of magnitude or two more dolphins than were detected in each patch. This suggests that spinner dolphin foraging is limited by time and consequently efficiency, rather than by the availability of food. The diel migration behavior of the mesopelagic boundary community probably eliminates the possibility of daytime foraging by spinner dolphins and may alter the costs of foraging throughout the night as prey depth and prey density change over the migration. Analysis of the effects of including even small incremental foraging costs in the model reveals that foraging by spinner dolphins on small prey probably increases energetic requirements dramatically. Changes in the exact parameters used in the model change the absolute numbers predicted, but not the shape of the curve or the relative predictions. Because of the curve’s shape, pre-

443

dictions from these simple foraging cost models, regardless of the costs used, suggest that spinner dolphins should selectively forage on prey longer than about 6 cm. These predictions match well with the lengths of prey observed in stomach contents. The estimated total length of myctophids (based on partially digested remains) was 7.7–7.8 cm. Dolar et al. (2003) found five species of myctophids with average total lengths (estimated from standard lengths) of 6.3– 13.1 cm. The mean measured length of whole squid from the single spinner dolphin’s stomach examined in this article was 7.7 cm. The estimated total length of the other squid (based on partially digested remains) was 8.4–11.4 cm. The mean total length of squid (converted from mantle lengths) found in spinner dolphin stomachs in Hawaii by Norris and Dohl (1980) was about 7.8 cm and by Dolar et al. (2003) was 7.6 cm. The distribution of squid lengths measured in this study shows an inverse relationship to the size distribution of most boundary community animals; animals in the boundary community have a mean length of about 5 cm with a distribution skewed toward smaller rather than larger animals (Reid 1994). This suggests that spinner dolphins are selectively foraging on larger prey, rather than simply selecting animal sizes at random from the population. While the sample size for shrimp is small, both this study and Norris and Dohl (1980) found that shrimp in Hawaiian spinner dolphin stomachs were 4.8–5.2 cm in total length, somewhat smaller than predicted by the estimated foraging costs. Dolar et al. (2003) found similarly small shrimp in the stomach contents of spinner dolphins in the eastern tropical Pacific. Perhaps the slow swimming speed of shrimp compared with other groups reduces the costs associated with foraging upon shrimp. Differences between taxonomic groups in the size distribution of the prey may also be a factor. No matter what size prey spinner dolphins consume from the size distribution available, the relatively small size of all available prey probably means that to meet their energetic needs, spinner dolphins need to forage quickly. If spinner dolphins use the entire time the mesopelagic layer is estimated to be within their foraging range (Fitch and Brownell 1968), they would need to eat at an average rate of 1.25 10-cm-long prey/min for maintenance. The foraging rates estimated for maintenance increase dramatically if smaller prey are consumed, up to 47 2.5-cm-long prey/min if foraging costs (both search and capture) are considered. If spinner dolphins consistently consume prey with the average lengths observed in this and the Norris and Dohl studies (1980), that is, just under 8 cm in total length, then a dolphin would need to average 3 to 4 prey/min for the entire 11-h potential foraging time. While it is unlikely that this rate is constant over time, particularly since the depth of the boundary layer changes by 100 m or more, it does indicate that spinner dolphins are time limited. This limitation lends support to the idea that spinner dolphins need to follow both inshore/upward and offshore/downward migrations of their prey (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a).

The total available energy in the mesopelagic boundary community is substantial and exceeds the needs of the population of spinner dolphins by orders of magnitude. Even an individual prey patch can support many times more dolphins than are actively foraging within it. This huge excess of energy suggests that density of prey rather than the total available biomass probably determines the density, and consequently the population size, of spinner dolphins. Changes in density of prey probably have strong effects on the foraging efficiency (i.e., prey capture rate) of spinner dolphins. This is evidenced by the strong positive correlation between spinner dolphins and areas of high prey density both vertically and horizontally (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). It appears that spinner dolphins actively increase the density of their prey through group foraging behavior that decreases the size of prey patches while increasing their aggregation (Benoit-Bird and Au 2003a). For this behavior to be adaptive, this investment of time by spinner dolphins to create prey areas with favorable characteristics must be outweighed by improvements in foraging efficiency within those prey areas. The estimates of foraging costs and needs presented here are based on limited information. While absolute predictions about spinner dolphin foraging cannot be made, the results point the way for future research testing both the expected results and the important assumptions. The effects of exploring a range of values in these calculations provide insight into the constraints under which spinner dolphins forage. A mammal with relatively high energetic needs, foraging on small mobile prey individually, but with a limited time to forage, must forage efficiently, both in terms of capture rate and selectivity. The importance of food to spinner dolphins and the effects of these constraints on the patterns observed in their nocturnal behavior are apparent. Spinner dolphins must be efficient at exploiting their food resources to meet their energetic needs. Acknowledgements Marlee Breese, Paul Nachtigall, and Robert Braun of the Hawaiian Islands Stranding Response Group provided information on the stranded animal and extracted and saved its stomach under Dr. Braun’s Letter of Authorization from the National Marine Fisheries Service. Iris Fischer, Michiel Schotten, Marc Lammers, Whitlow Au, and Ariel Rivera-Vicente assisted in the dissection of the stomach. Timothy Tricas generously permitted me to utilize his laboratory and Ariel Rivera-Vicente made sure I had everything I needed. Gayla Ivey and Ken Longenecker assisted with the classification of fish otoliths. Whitlow Au, Paul Nachtigall, and George Losey provided helpful comments on earlier drafts of this manuscript. Support for this work was provided by a Leonida Family Scholarship, the ARCS Foundation, and the Hawaii Institute of Marine Biology’s Marine Mammal Research Program. This work was also funded by a grant from the National Oceanic and Atmospheric Administration, Project R/FM-7, which is sponsored by the University of Hawaii Sea Grant College Program, SOEST, under Institutional Grant No. NA16RG2254 from NOAA Office of Sea Grant, Department of Commerce. The views expressed herein are those of the author and do not necessarily reflect the views of NOAA or any of its subagencies. UNIHI-SEAGRANT-JC-03–01 This is HIMB contribution 1172.

444

References Acevedo-Gutierrez A, Croll DA, Tershy BR (2002) High feeding costs limit dive time in the largest whales. J Exp Biol 205:1747– 1753 Arnaya IN, Sano N, Iida K (1989a) Studies on acoustic target strength of squid II. Effect of behaviour on averaged dorsal aspect target strength. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 40:83–99 Arnaya IN, Sano N, Iida K (1989b) Studies on acoustic target strength of squid III. Measurement of the mean target strength of small live squid. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 40:110 Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2001) Target strength measurements of animals from the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary community. J Acoust Soc Am 110:812–819 Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2002) Energy: converting from acoustic to biological resource units. J Acoust Soc Am 111:2070–2075 Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2003a) Prey dynamics affect foraging by a pelagic predator (Stenella longirostris) over a range of spatial and temporal scales. Behav Ecol Sociobiol 53:364–373 Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL (2003b) Spatial dynamics of a nearshore micronekton sound-scattering layer. ICES J Mar Sci 60:899– 913 Benoit-Bird KJ, Au WWL, Brainard RE, Lammers MO (2001) Diel horizontal migration of the Hawaiian mesopelagic boundary community observed acoustically. Mar Ecol Prog Ser 217:1–14 Brooks AH (1977) A study of the swimbladders of selected mesopelagic fish species. In: Andersen NR, Zahuranec BJ (eds) Oceanic sound scattering prediction. Plenum Press, New York, pp 565–590 Bunce A (2001) Prey consumption of Australasian gannets (Morus serrator) breeding in Port Phillip Bay, southeast Australia, and potential overlap with commercial fisheries. ICES J Mar Sci 58:904–915 Burke BJ, Rice JA (2002) A linked foraging and bioenergetics model for southern flounder. Trans Am Fish Soc 131:120–131 Cadenat J, Doutre M (1959) Notes sur les delphinides Ouest-Africains, 5. Sur un Prodelphinus a long bec capture au large des cotes du Senegal: Prodelphinus longirostris. Bull Inst Fondam Afr Noire 21:787–792 Clarke M, Young RE (1998) Description and analysis of cephalopod beaks from stomachs of six species of odontocete cetaceans stranded on Hawaiian shores. J Mar Biol Assoc UK 78:623–641 Cowles DL (2001) Swimming speed and metabolic rate during routine swimming and simulated diel vertical migration of Sergestes similis in the laboratory. Pac Sci 55:215–226 Cox M, Gaglione E, Prowten P, Noonan M (1996) Food preferences communicated via symbol discrimination by a California sea lion (Zalophus californianus). Aquat Mamm 22:3–10 Day DE, Mintz EM, Bartness TJ (2002) Diet choice exaggerates food hoarding, intake and pup survival across reproduction. Physiol Behav 75:143–157 Dolar MLL, Walker WA, Kooyman GL, Perrin WF (2003) Comparative feeding ecology of spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) and Fraser’s dolphins (Lagenodelphis hosei) in the Sulu Sea. Mar Mamm Sci 19:1–19 Dufour DL, Reina JC, Spurr GB (2002) Energy intake and expenditure of free-living, lactating Colombian women in an urban setting. Eur J Clin Nutr 56:205–213 Fitch JE, Brownell RL Jr (1968) Fish otoliths and their importance in interpreting feeding habits. J Fish Res Bd Can 25:2561–2574 Gorman ML, Mills MG, Raath JP, Speakman JR (1998) High hunting costs make African wild dogs vulnerable to kleptoparasitism by hyaenas. Nature 391:479–481 Honda S, Wada T, Matsubara T (1992) Relationship between food consumption and growth of Japanese sardine Sardinops melanostictus in the laboratory. Bull Fac Fish Hokkaido Univ 56:1–6

Kastelein RA, Elst CA van der, Tennant HK, Wiepkema PR (2000) Food consumption and growth of a female dusky dolphin (Lagenorhynchus obscurus). Zoo Biol 19:131–142 Kastelein RA, Au WWL, de Haan D (2002a) Underwater audiogram of a striped dolphin (Stenella coeruleoalba) measured with narrow-band frequency-modulated signals. J Acoust Soc Am 112:334–344 Kastelein RA, Vaughan N, Walton S, Wiepkema PR (2002b) Food intake and body measurements of Atlantic bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) in captivity. Mar Environ Res 53:199–218 Kils U (1979) Swimming speed and escape capacity of Antarctic krill, Euphausia superba. Rep Mar Res 27:264–266 Kizevetter IV (1973) Chemistry and technology of Pacific fish. The Israel Program for Scientific Translations (trans). U.S. Department of Commerce, National Technical Information Service, Springfield, Va., ‘IT72–50019 (Original work published 1971) MacLennan DN, Simmonds EJ (1992) Fisheries acoustics. Chapman and Hall, New York Martensson PE, Lager Gotaas AR, Nordoy ES, Blix AS (1996) Seasonal changes in energy density of prey of northeast Atlantic seals and whales. Mar Mamm Sci 12:635–640 Norris KS, Dohl TP (1980) Behavior of the Hawaiian spinner dolphin, Stenella longirostris. Fish Bull 77:821–849 Norris KS, Wu¨rsig B, Wells RS, Wu¨rsig M (1994) The Hawaiian spinner dolphin. University of California Press, Berkeley, Calif. Perrin WF, Warner RR, Fiscus CH, Holts DB (1973) Stomach contents of porpoise, Stenella spp., and yellowfin tuna, Thunnus albacares, in mixed-species aggregations. Fish Bull 71:1077– 1092 Perrin WF, Dolar MLL, Robineau D (1999) Spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) of the western Pacific and southeast Asia: Pelagic and shallow-water forms. Mar Mamm Sci 15:1029–1053 Reid SB (1994) Spatial structure of the mesopelagic fish community in the Hawaiian boundary region. Ph.D. dissertation, Department of Oceanography, University of Hawaii, Honolulu Reid SB, Hirota J, Young RE, Hallacher LE (1991) Mesopelagicboundary community in Hawaii: micronekton at the interface between neritic and oceanic ecosystems. Mar Biol 109:427–440 Sambilay VC Jr (1990) Interrelationships between swimming speed, caudal fin aspect ratio and body length of fishes. Fishbyte 83:16–20 Schotten M, Au WWL, Lammers MO, Aubauer R (2004) Echolocation recordings and localizations of wild spinner dolphins (Stenella longirostris) and pantropical spotted dolphins (Stenella attenuata) using a four hydrophone array. In: Thomas JA, Moss C, Vater M (eds) Echolocation in bats and dolphins. University of Chicago Press, Chicago (in press) Smith RL, Paul JM, Gillispie J (1997) Energy content of Arctic cod and saffron cod in the northeastern Chukchi Sea. In: Reynolds JB (ed) Fish ecology in Arctic North America. American Fisheries Society, Symposium 19. American Fisheries Society, Bethesda, Md., pp 319–325 Stockwell JD, Johnson BM (1999) Field evaluation of a bioenergetics-based foraging model for kokanee (Oncorhynchus nerka). Can J Fish Aquat Sci 56:140–151 Urick RJ (1983) Principles of underwater sound. McGraw Hill, New York Williams TM, Haun JE, Friedl W (1999) The diving physiology of bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus) I. Balancing the demands of exercise for energy conservation at depth. J Exp Biol 202:2739–2748 Yatsu A, Yamanaka K, Yamashiro C (1999) Tracking experiments of the jumbo flying squid, Dosidicus gigas, with an ultrasonic telemetry system in the eastern Pacific Ocean. Bull Natl Res Inst Far Seas Fish 36:55–60 Yazdi P, Kilian A, Culik BM (1999) Energy expenditure of swimming bottlenose dolphins (Tursiops truncatus). Mar Biol 134:601–607

Related Documents

Benoit Bird 2004
November 2019 13
Benoit Bird 2003
November 2019 4
Benoit-bird Et Al
November 2019 11
Benoit-bird And Au 2003
November 2019 2
Benoit
August 2019 22
Benoit Mandelbrot
June 2020 21