Violence & Effects On Children

  • Uploaded by: Jacobin Parcelle
  • 0
  • 0
  • December 2019
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Violence & Effects On Children as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 3,557
  • Pages: 4
Vietnamese Reolution, The Luong H V 1992 Reolution in the Village: Tradition and Transformation in North Vietnam, 1925–1988. University of Hawaii Press, Honolulu, HI Marr D G 1971 Vietnamese Anticolonialism, 1885–1925. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA Marr D G 1995 Vietnam 1945. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA Marx K 1964 The Eighteenth Brumaire of Louis Bonaparte. International Publishers, New York Mitchell E J 1968 Inequality and insurgency: A statistical study of South Vietnam. World Politics 20: 421–38 Murray M J 1980 The Deelopment of Capitalism in Colonial Indochina. University of California Press, Berkeley Paige J M 1975 Agrarian Reolution: Social Moements and Export Agriculture in the Underdeeloped World. Free Press, New York Paige J M 1983 Social theory and peasant revolution in Vietnam and Guatemala. Theory and Society 12: 699–737 Popkin S 1979 The Rational Peasant: The Political Economy of Rural Society in Vietnam. University of California Press, Berkeley, CA Scott J C 1976 The Moral Economy of the Peasant. Yale University Press, New Haven, CT Tai H T H 1983 Millenarianism and Peasant Politics in Vietnam. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA Tai H T H 1992 Radicalism and the Origin of the Vietnamese Reolution. Harvard University Press, Cambridge, MA Toan N K, Binh N C, Tao V, Nam P X, Thanh B D 1985 Lich su Viet Nam (History of Vietnam), Vol. 2 Nha xuat ban Khoa hoc xa hoi, Hanoi, Viet Nam Wolf E R 1969 Peasant Wars of the Twentieth Century. Harper and Row, New York Woodside A B 1976 Community and Reolution in Modern Vietnam. Houghton Mifflin, Boston

H. V. Luong

Violence and Effects on Children 1. Violence in America: History The common dictionary definition of violence runs something like the following: ‘exertion of physical force so as to injure or abuse.’ The inclusion of ‘abuse’ in the definition is important, because it opens up the issues of force being used to induce negative psychological states and to violate basic human dignity. Thus, violence includes the range of behaviors that assault or batter, that induce fear or terror, and that demean and humiliate. Understanding the effects of violence on children demands an interdisciplinary perspective, one that incorporates psychological, biological, sociological, anthropological, and historical insights within the context of a larger ecological model. Anchoring this issue is the fact that each year in the United States nearly 5,000 children and youth commit acts of lethal violence, half against others (murder) and half against themselves (suicide). The numbers of children and 16184

youth who commit potentially lethal acts is, of course, many times greater, what with the tens of thousands of ‘serious assaults’ and ‘suicide attempts’ that take place each year involving minors. For each of these kids, and for the families, communities and societies of which they are a part, this problem of violence constitutes a crisis. The current crisis of these kids is not historically new, however. Writing in Juenile Justice Update in 1997, Robert Shepherd points out that the New York Times 140 years ago editorially bemoaned the fact that ‘the number of boy burglars, boy robbers and boy murderers is so astoundingly large as to alarm all good men.’ He offered the following analysis from a child psychiatrist working in Manhattan for the Juvenile Court in the 1950s: ‘At first it comes as a shock to meet youngsters under 16 who rob at the point of a gun, push dope, rape and kill. I’ve seen boys of 7 so small they could barely clear the desk who had sold themselves to sex perverts. Others had shot out kids eyes or had clubbed or knifed them, just for the fun of it.’ And the statement is from an article entitled Manhattan’s Child Criminals Are My Job in the Saturday Eening Post of March 27, 1954. Recall that in the mid-nineteenth century, the governor of New York had to call out the militia to deal with rampant youth gangs in New York City.

2. Violence in America: Today While there are historical dimensions to the current situation, there are features of the contemporary situation that are distinctive as well. For one thing, we must recognize that the magnitude of the problem is different today. There has been a seven-fold increase in per capita aggravated assault rates among youth in the United States since 1956. Also, kids are being tried as adults and incarcerated correspondingly, in what seems like a macabre return to the medieval concepts of children as simply little adults. But the parallel is even more subject to documentation than that. The homicide rate in the city of Amsterdam in the year 1450 is estimated to have been 150 per hundred thousand. By 1850, when Amsterdam had been ‘civilized,’ the rate dropped to 2 per hundred thousand. This rate might be taken as a kind of base line for the minimum expectable homicide rate, the rate we find in most of the modern and relatively affluent countries in the world. But in the US we continue to find rates of 20 per hundred thousand and in the most oppressed social environments within our society we find rates of 160 per hundred thousand! Homicide rates provide only an imprecise indicator of the overall problem of violence in the lives of American children and youth, however, for behind each murder stand many nonlethal assaults. This ratio varies as a function of both medical trauma technology (which prevents assaults from becoming homicides)

Violence and Effects on Children and weapons technology (which can increase or decrease the lethality of assaults). An example from Chicago illustrates this: the city’s homicide rate in 1973 and 1993 was approximately the same, and yet the rate of serious assault had increased approximately 400 percent during that period. Thus, the ratio of assaults to homicides increased substantially—from 1:100 in 1973 to 1:400 in 1993 (Garbarino et al. 1992). Class, race, and gender exert important influences on exposure to community violence. The odds of being a homicide victim range from 1:21 for Black males, to 1:369 for white females—with white males at 1:13 and Black females at 1:104 (Bell and Jenkins 1993). Being an American itself is a risk factor. As noted above, the US far exceeds all other modern industrialized nations in its youth homicide rate (even for young whites, where the rate of 11.2 per 100,000 is far more than the second place country, Scotland, with 5 per 100,000 (Richters and Martinez 1993).

3. Exposure to Violence as an Influence on Deelopment Having established this historical and social context, what next becomes clear is that understanding the effects of violence on children is very much a matter of recognizing the developmental links between early experiences of violence as a victim and later experiences of violence as a perpetrator. This developmental relationship exists both within the lives of individual children and across the generations in families. Research indicates that inside virtually every dangerously violent youth is an untreated traumatized child, a child with experiences of violent victimization, and that the there is substantial cross-generational continuity in violence within families. Research by psychiatrists Perry et al. (1995) and van der Kolk (1996) has begun to illuminate the neurological processes that translate early trauma into later dangerous behavior through brain development and neurochemistry. Dodge et al. (1997) have further elaborated this link by documenting the adaptive processes that link the experience of violence as a young child to the development of a pattern of aggressive antisocial behavior as an elementary school-age child. Their research reveals that the problems comes when abused children develop four particular psychological adaptations (hypersensitivity to negative social cues, obliviousness to positive social cues, a readily accessible repertoire of aggressive behavior, and a belief that aggression is a successful strategy in social relations). Such children are seven times more likely than abused children who do not develop these patterns to end upon diagnosed with ‘conduct disorder’ (chronic aggressive and antisocial behavior). Abused children who do not develop these

patterns are no more likely that children in general to develop conduct disorder.

4. Children in War Zones This is highly significant because it is from the ranks of children with conduct disorder that most chronic violent delinquents come (about a third of such children emerge as chronic violent delinquents according to a variety of studies—cf. Loeber and Farrington 1997). Child abuse and community violence are rampant in the lives of kids who become violent youth. This is one of the most important effects of violence on children. My own research illuminates these links through intensive, multiple interviews with boys involved in lethal violence (Garbarino 1999), and highlights the importance of geographic concentrations of violence that create ‘war zone’ like settings for children. These are the social settings in which the rates of child victimization is highest (with child maltreatment rates many times higher than in other neighborhoods—Garbarino et al. 1992) and from which most lethal youth violence comes (Snyder and Sickmund 1999). Children living in neighborhoods which simulate war zones are exposed to high levels of violence. A survey of sixth to 10th graders in New Haven, Connecticut, revealed that 40 percent had witnessed at least one incident of violent crime within the previous 12 months (Marans and Cohen 1993). In three high risk neighborhoods in Chicago, 17 percent of the elementary school age children had witnessed domestic violence, 31 percent had seen someone shot, and, 84 percent had seen someone beat up (Bell and Jenkins 1993). Some 30 percent of the kids living in high crime neighborhoods of major metropolitan areas like Chicago have witnessed a homicide by the time they are 15 years old, and more than 70 percent have witnessed a serious assault. These figures are much more like the experience of kids in the war zones we have visited in other countries (Garbarino et al. 1991) than they are of what we should expect for children, living in ‘peace.’ Richters and Martinez (1993) have amplified these results. In their study, 43 percent of the fifth and sixth graders had witnessed a mugging in a ‘moderately violent’ neighborhood in Washington, DC. Other researchers echo these findings (e.g., Taylor et al. 1994).

5. Children Adapt: For Better and For Worse Children adapt to their perception of community safety in many important ways. One is their view of the future. A Harris poll of sixth to 12th graders in 1992 revealed that 35 percent worried they would not live to old age because they would be shot (Harris et al. 1994). In our interviews with families living in public housing 16185

Violence and Effects on Children projects in Chicago we learned that virtually all the children had first-hand experiences with shooting by the time they were five years old (Dubrow and Garbarino 1989). A six-year-old girl once said that her job was to find her two-year-old sister whenever the shooting started and get her to safety in the bathtub of their apartment. ‘The bathroom is the safest place,’ she said. Interviews with school-age children and youth confirm that the ‘gun culture’ is a potent factor in the life of children in diverse settings in the US (Garbarino 1995, 1999). Teen use of guns in homicides increased from 64 percent in 1987 to 78 percent in 1991. During the same period, teen arrests for crimes involving weapons increased 62 percent (Snyder and Sickmund 1995). When children understand that adults cannot protect them they are left with what we might call juvenile vigilantism, the impulse to protect themselves, to take up weapons and relationships that substitute for adult protection. This is evident in comments from children and youth such as ‘If I join a gang I’m 50 percent safe. If I don’t join a gang I’m zero percent safe’ (Garbarino 1999). Adults don’t enter into the equation.

7. Future Directions As research on the impact of violence on children matures it turns increasingly to studies of the conditions under which alternative pathways are taken by affected children. Why do some child victims become adolescent perpetrators while others do not? How can early intervention change these pathways? How significant are the neurobiological effects of early trauma? These and other questions will require many years of sustained research to address adequately. Unfortunately, the world shows no signs of preventing childhood exposure to violence, so these questions must be answered. See also: Childhood Sexual Abuse and Risk for Adult Psychopathology; Domestic Violence: Sociological Perspectives; Early Childhood: Socioemotional Risks; Violence in Anthropology; Violence: Public; Street Children: Psychological Perspectives; War, Political Violence and their Psychological Effects on Children: Cultural Concerns

6. Risk Accumulates The key to understanding the effects of living in a war zone is the recognition that these children do not simply face a single threat to development, a solitary risk factor. Rather, they face multiple risk factors, a fact of overarching importance in light of research by Sameroff et al. (1987) documenting the cumulative effect of risk factors. In Sameroff ’s study, while the presence of one or two risk factors was generally manageable for children, the presence of three or more risk factors was associated with significant developmental impairment. These are the children of greatest concern to us, the children who face major accumulations of risk factors, and who are thus most at risk for the psychological effects of trauma due to violence. Tolan (1996) examined this phenomenon in detail in a study in a Chicago-based study in which he asked the question, ‘What percent of kids are resilient if we measure resilience as neither requiring mental health intervention or remedial education?’ When he asked that question of data from boys growing up in the most afflicted war zone neighborhoods of Chicago, living in abusive and impoverished families, contending with minority status in a racist society, and looked at this kids over a two-year period, the answer was zero percent. None of the children by age 15 were resilient in Tolan’s terms. This testifies to the effects of violence when they occur within a larger context of social and psychological risk accumulation. The relentless pressure imposed on kids who come from that nexus of community violence, family disruption, and personal experience of trauma is uniformly overwhelming. 16186

Bibliography Bell C C, Jenkins E J 1993 Traumatic stress and children. Journal of Health Care for the Poor and Undersered 2(1): 175–85 Dodge K A, Pettit G S, Bates J E 1997 How the experience of early physical abuse leads children to become chronically aggressive. In: Cicchetti D, Toth S L (eds.) Deelopmental Perspecties on Trauma: Theory, Research, and Interention. University of Rochester Press, Rochester, NY, pp. 263–88 Dubrow N F, Garbarino J 1989 Living in the war zone: Mothers and young children in public housing development. Journal of Child Welfare 68: 3–20 Garbarino J 1995 Raising Children in a Socially Toxic Enironment. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA Garbarino J 1999 Lost Boys: Why Our Sons Turn Violent and How We Can Sae Them. Free Press, New York Garbarino J, Dubrow N, Kostelny K, Pardo C 1992 Children in Danger: Coping with the Consequences of Community Violence. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA Garbarino J, Kostelny K, Dubrow N 1991 No Place to be a Child: Growing up in a War Zone. Jossey-Bass, San Francisco, CA Harris et al. 1994 Metropolitan Life Sureys of the American Teacher: Violence in America’s Public Schools: Part II. Metropolitan Life Insurance, New York van der Kolk B A 1996 The body keeps score: Approaches to the psychobiology of posttraumatic stress disorder. In: van der Kolk B A, McFarlane A C, Weisaeth L (eds.) Traumatic Stress: The Effects of Oerwhelming Experience on Mind, Body, and Society. Guilford Press, New York, pp. 214–41 Loeber R, Farrington D P 1997 Strategies and yields of longitudinal studies on antisocial behavior. In: Stoff D M, Breiling J, Maser J D (eds.) Handbook of Antisocial Behaior. John Wiley & Sons, New York, pp. 125–39 Marans S, Cohen D J 1993 Children and inner-city violence: Strategies for intervention. In: Leavitt L A, Fox N A (eds.) The

Violence and Media Psychological Effects of War and Violence on Children. Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Hillsdale, NJ, pp. 281–301 Snyder H N, Sickmund M Juenile Offender and Victims: A National Report. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Washington, DC Perry B D, Pollard R A, Blakley T L, Baker W L, Vigilante D 1995 Childhood trauma, the neurobiology of adaptation, and ‘‘use-dependent’’ development of the brain: How ‘‘states’’ become ‘‘traits’’. Infant Mental Health Journal 16(4): 271–91 Richters J E, Martinez P 1993 The NIMH Community Violence Project: I. Children as victims of and witnesses to violence. Psychiatry: Interpersonal and Biological Processes 56(1): 7–21 Sameroff A, Seifer R, Barocas R, Zax M 1987 Intelligence Quotient scores of 4-year-old children: Social environment risk factors. Pediatrics 79: 343–50 Shepherd R 1997 Juenile Justice Update. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Washington, DC Snyder H N, Sickmund M 1999 Juenile Offenders and Victims: 1999 National Report. Office of Juvenile Justice and Delinquency Prevention, Washington, DC Taylor L, Zuckerman B, Harik V, Groves B M 1994 Witnessing violence by young children and their mothers. Journal of Deelopmental and Behaioral Pediatrics 15(2): 120–23 Tolan P H 1996 How resilient is the concept of resilience? Community Psychologist 29(4): 12–15

J. Garbarino Copyright # 2001 Elsevier Science Ltd. All rights reserved.

Violence and Media Public and academic concern about media’s contribution to real world violence are about as old as the mass media and the social sciences themselves (Wartella and Reeves 1985). Despite frequent framing of the matter as ‘controversial,’ extensive research—an estimated 3,000 (Donnerstein et al. 1994) to 3,500 (Wartella et al. 1998) studies in the United States alone—have examined the impact of media violence, and a number of recent major reviews (Huston et al. 1992, Murray 1994, see also Potter 1999, Paik and Comstock 1994, Comstock and Paik 1991), have concluded that media violence plays a measurable role in real-world violence. A variety of US agencies, including the Centers for Disease Control of the US Public Health Service (1991), and medical and public interest organizations, including the American Medical Association, the American Academy of Pediatrics, and the National Conference of Parent-Teacher Associations, have identified media violence as a public health problem. The review below focuses most heavily on US research and US media, most notably American television, primarily because a large majority of the published social science research on media and violence is US research on American audiovisual media. Potter (1999, pp. 44–5), for example, reports 42 published content analyses of US television since 1954, and just 19 from the rest of the world. Moreover, American media are among the world’s most violent—and most exported—and real-world violence

is a recurring public policy issue. Further, much of this literature concerns impacts of media violence on children and adolescents, for the inter-related reasons that young audiences are considered the most impressionable and most vulnerable. Adults are generally viewed to be more resistant to the deleterious influences of violence, and, as some would argue (cf. Huesmann 1997), violent behaviors in adulthood may be traced to media use during childhood.

1. Theories of Effect Three models have been proposed to describe the process by which such learning and imitation of media violence occurs: social learning theory, priming effects theory, and a social developmental model of learning (Wartella et al. 1998). First proposed by Albert Bandura in the 1960s, social learning theory is the best known theoretical account of violence effects. Bandura asserts that through observing television models, viewers come to learn behaviors which are appropriate, that is, which behaviors will be rewarded and which punished. In this way, viewers seek to attain rewards and therefore imitate these media models. When both children and adults are shown an aggressive model who is either rewarded or punished for their aggressive behavior, models who are positively reinforced influence imitation among the viewers. Even research in the field has demonstrated that aggression is learned at a young age and becomes more impervious to change as the child grows older. In a longitudinal study to examine the long-term effects of television violence on aggression and criminal behavior, Huesmann et al. (1984) studied a group of youth across 22 years, at ages 8, 18, and 30. For boys (and to a lesser, though still significant extent for girls), early television violence viewing correlated with self-reported aggression at age 30 and added significantly to the prediction of serious criminal arrests accumulated by age 30. These researchers find a longitudinal relationship between habitual childhood exposure to television violence and adult crime and suggest that approximately 10 percent of the variability in later criminal behavior can be attributed to television violence. Priming effects theory serves to augment the more traditional social learning theory account of television violence effects. In the work of Leonard Berkowitz and his colleagues, this theoretical account asserts that many media effects are immediate, transitory, and short-term (Berkowitz 1984). Berkowitz suggests that when people watch television violence, it activates or ‘primes’ other semantically related thoughts which may influence how the person responds to the violence on television. Viewers who identify with the actors on television may imagine themselves like that character carrying out the aggressive actions of the character on television, and research evidence suggests that exposure to media aggression does indeed ‘prime’ other 16187

International Encyclopedia of the Social & Behavioral Sciences

ISBN: 0-08-043076-7

Related Documents


More Documents from ""

December 2019 58
Balance Of Power Political,
December 2019 56
Arms Control
December 2019 58