Memory Wksp Ii

  • October 2019
  • PDF

This document was uploaded by user and they confirmed that they have the permission to share it. If you are author or own the copyright of this book, please report to us by using this DMCA report form. Report DMCA


Overview

Download & View Memory Wksp Ii as PDF for free.

More details

  • Words: 2,500
  • Pages: 6
Memorization and the Performing Musician

November, 2007

Conscious memorization is the period after which piece can be played technically correct with notation, but before a piece can be performed without music.





Memorization begins before & continues after conscious memorization Three stages  Preview  Practice  Overlearning



 Memorizing music involves mindful and deliber-ate practice. Developing an understanding of the music and recognizing patterns are active processes; thoughtlessly repeating muscular movements is a passive process.

Enculturation

Can memorize piece without understanding relationships (passive)  Novice musicians  Mindless repetition – not efficient nor

stable    

Understanding a piece requires active processing Patterns unique to individual performer Labels beneficial, but not necessary Younger students need guidance to patterns

Mishra, J. (2005). A theoretical model of musical memory. Psychomusicology, 19(1), 75-89. Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston



Analytical “memory”  Also termed

 Analytical “memory” requires the cognitive interpretation of patterns and repetitions in a musical work. The focus is on forming relationships and finding familiar patterns.

November, 2007



Conceptual memory

Not a “memory”, but a process  Meaningful understanding of the piece  Need not be theoretical analysis  Need not be through score study



Jennifer Mishra

Differs from sensory memorization styles  influenced largely by training and ability  Expert and advanced musicians use; inexperienced performers do not

University of Houston

November, 2007

Human mind designed to find patterns.

 Musical pattern is any meaningful grouping of notes  Learned patterns e.g., scales, arpeggios, chords  Found relationships between 2+ notes  Pattern processed as unit, not individual notes  Patterns allow for prediction  Sight reading  If memory lapse occurs “Man is, perhaps above all else, a predicting animal…He must pattern the world… we must pattern the world.” Music, The Arts & Ideas Leonard Meyer (p. 227-8 )

 Structural boundaries (phrase structure)

emerging as dominant in experts. Jennifer Mishra



University of Houston

November, 2007

Jennifer Mishra

Music performed serially, but understood as a web of connections



 Web is unique to the piece and to the individual performer  Patterns are not what composer intended, but what performer perceives  Performer’s understanding of piece impacts interpretation (Consciously or unconsciously)  Can find unique patterns not intended by composer.  Even aleatoric music, truly random music, can have



patterns imposed upon it by performer 

University of Houston

November, 2007

Labels- brings patterns to the surface, fixes in mind  Isolate elements to direct attention to patterns.

Mapping is visual analysis & simplification of structure  Visual icons symbolizing patterns  Verbalizing or labeling patterns  Attending to patterns

 

Similarities can be unconsciously understood, but labeling brings to forefront Need not use theoretical terms/labels

Patterns & connections can be anywhere Shockley, R. P. (2001). Mapping Music: For Faster Learning and Secure Memory. A-R Publications.

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

November, 2007

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

1

Memorization and the Performing Musician

November, 2007



Common practice techniques emphasize pattern recognition; allow obscure patterns to become clear      

 

playing hands separately blocking chords (on keyboard) rehearsing under-tempo rehearsing the piece backwards score study away from the instrument transposing

Analysis may be visual (based on scored notation) Analysis may be aural (i.e., not notation-based)  interplay between voices in a fugue

 Shockley, R. P. (2001). Mapping Music: For Faster Learning and Secure Memory. A-R Publications. Jennifer Mishra University of Houston November, 2007

Analysis may be kinesthetic (e.g., blocking chords)

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

 

1900

Most advocate a mixture of memorization styles  implying memories are equal and mutually supporting  No research support  Isolated usage of strategies rare (depending on study)





Not “memories”, but processing strategies

Aural “memory” is the ability to hear the notes of a piece of music in the proper order without relying on a sound source or notation.



Used primarily to monitor performance for errors Good aural memory doesn’t insure good performance

 can hear the next note and not know how

to play it



Removing aural feedback doesn’t affect performance

 Pianists can perform in absence of

auditory and kinesthetic feedback (Repp, 1999)

 Can be used as early as sight reading  Used throughout notation-based practice 

Also termed

 melodic memory, auditory memory, ear

memory

Four “memories” popular memorization topic  Focus of 60% of 121 pedagogical articles on memory since



November, 2007

 Only effect when aural feedback removed

Aural, Visual, & Kinesthetic

was pianists pedaled less (Repp, 1998)

 Sensory Learning styles – rather than memory stores Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston



November, 2007

Jennifer Mishra

Also termed

University of Houston



 Photographic, eye memory  Visual “memory” is the ability to recall a mental picture of the musical notation, as a whole or in parts, or to visualize finger patterns or hand positions.

Not necessarily full-scale photographic memory for notation  Visualize part of notation  Visualize finger patterns on an instrument  Isolated visual memory trainable



Used primarily as a memory cue to initiate recall  beginnings of sections  difficult sections

Also termed  Motor, hand, tactile, muscular, finger,

Kinesthetic “memory” is the retention of muscular movements involved in performing a piece of music. Tactile memory is the memory for the feel of the instrument and is not exactly the same as kinesthetic memory.

Jennifer Mishra

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

November, 2007

digital memory 

Most misunderstood “memory”  Often confused with automated

procedural memory (more later)  Not incidentally developed  Not “thoughtless” 

Used to assist with difficult or awkward passages

University of Houston

November, 2007

2

Memorization and the Performing Musician



Research attempt to determine usage among musicians     



November, 2007

 

Mixed results Aural: 4 – 42% Visual: 0 – 50% Kinesthetic: 3 – 50% Mixed: 13 – 58%



Variable definitions for memory styles Question too broad “which memory style do you use?” Musical Memory Inventory (MMI)  Specific questions  Asked “how often”

Discrepancy - lack of standard definitions

(Analytical not considered in all studies, so omitted) Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

 

Past experiences affect the way we learn new material  Every musical experience combines into schemas  Expectations formed  Applied to new music

Musical Memory Inventory (MMI) results

Mishra, J. (2007). Correlating Musical Memorization Styles and Perceptual Learning Modalities. Visions of Research in Music Education, 9-10. Retrieved July 14, 2007, from www.rider.edu/~vrme/



Continually processing and storing information  Learning all the time  Learning without conscious study  Can be problematic - CDM



Memorize, at least in part, naturally through experience and practice

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

 Find the error (recording 

is performed as musically expected, not as notated)

Tonal music easier to memorize than atonal  More experience with tonal music =

stronger expectations  20th century music often played with

notation 

Structure preserving errors (“proofreader’s errors”)

Error in: Breitkopf Edition Henle Urtext Edition Peters Edition

 Goldovsky’s Sightreading Experiment

Wolf, T. (1976). A cognitive model of musical sightreading. Journal Psycholinguistic Research, 5(2), 143-171. Jennifer of Mishra University of Houston

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

Example from Boulez Third Piano Sonata November, 2007

Brahms Capriccio Op. 76 No. 2 Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston November, 2007

3

Memorization and the Performing Musician



November, 2007

Memorization begins very early in the learning process  Unconscious  Even when memorization not primary goal



Sightreading study  4 playings - mask bars  Between 30 and 88 % of the missing notes could be

recalled

Musicians often decide whether they are “good” or “poor” memorizers based on a handful of salient experiences or informal conversations rather than a systematic observation and comparison. There is no doubt that some musicians memorize faster than others; however even the most advanced musician requires time to memorize



Many factors potentially influence the amount of time required to memorize  Characteristics of the musician  Experience - Enculturation  Memorization practice strategies

 Characteristics of the composition 

Memorization takes a long time  36-bar exercise – up to 100 minutes

Lehmann, A. C., & Ericsson, K. A. (1996). Performance without preparation: structure and acquisition of expert sight-reading and accompanying performance. Psychomusicology, 15, 1-29. Jennifer Mishra



University of Houston

November, 2007

Organizing memorization practice



 Holistic (Whole) – develop concept of entire piece  Segmented (Part) – break piece down into sections  Additive – systematically lengthen

Holistic - most efficient  Emphasizes connections throughout piece



Segmented & Serial – less efficient  “section” 2-100 bars

sections Serial – play as far as possible

 Other possible divisions: hands, elements  Structural, but not necessarily  By line – not musical structure

Mishra, J. (2002). A qualitative analysis of strategies employed in efficient and inefficient memorization. Bulletin of the Council for Research in Music Education, 152, 74-86. Jennifer Mishra



University of Houston

November, 2007

Why segmented strategy might be less efficient

Jennifer Mishra



 focused on discrete fragments ignoring others  very short segments of 2-4 measures – not necessarily

counter productive  Amount of segmentation  Difficulty of piece  Experience of performer (e.g., with genre)  Length of piece

 Boring, mindless repetition  Unable to connect newly mastered segment to as

previously learned segments had been forgotten!

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

November, 2007

Evidence from expert musicians - Alternating Holistic & Segmented  Musically meaningful segments

(43 consecutive times – without any discernable errors )

the discrete segments.

November, 2007

 mindless sectionalizing and rigid adherence to repetition is

musically meaningful  Repeat each fragment in isolation a large number of times

 Additional time was wasted on practicing the connection of

University of Houston



Intersperse Holistic practice to develop an overall concept of piece

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

4

Memorization and the Performing Musician

November, 2007

 

Serial – inefficient strategy  Beginning learned very well, but the end never played  musician conceded defeat when confronted by an error or

memory lapse by returning to the beginning  No attempt is made to understand why the error occurred or to place the problematic section into the context of the piece.  strategy regrettably may be in common usage, especially among younger, less experienced performers

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

Curve Estimation

    

 11.14 10.33 9.53

Total Memorization Time (Hours)



5.51 4.71 3.90

4

3.10

When is a piece memorized?

Overlearning Stage - point after which a piece can be performed from memory, but before the memory is stable enough for performance.  Important stage, though the purpose of extended practice

2.29

not well understood

1.49

2

November, 2007

memory

7.92 7.12 6.31

6

University of Houston

 No standardized operational definition for “memorized.”  The first time a piece played through by memory? Second?  Musicians continue to rehearse even after piece played by

8.72

8

Number of notes single best predictor of memorization time

Jennifer Mishra

11.94

10

length of the piece (in terms of notes) harmonic complexity Tonality familiarity with the genre

 Pianists more time  Pianists more notes

14

12

Compositional characteristics of a piece, to a large extent, determines the amount of time required to memorize the piece

0.69

 As much as 150% more practice time is needed to stabilize

0 100

200

300

400

500

600

700

800

900

1000

1100 1200

1300

the memory

1400 1500

Number of Notes

Note: Times are only measured until 1 or 2 memorized playthroughs Jennifer Mishra

 

University of Houston

November, 2007

Overlearning = Testing Testing memory cues (reminders, triggers) for effectiveness

Jennifer Mishra

 

 

November, 2007

Structural boundaries as landmarks Hierarchy of retrieval  Entire piece not retrieved simultaneously - lightening the

memory load

 Testing cues - starting at various points throughout the    

University of Houston

piece Cues not resulting in stable retrieval can be replaced Unnecessary cues removed Added cues especially in difficult sections Fewest cues possible = less to remember



Test by starting at points throughout piece  Structural boundaries  Not random points, cues are related to musical form

Cues may be aural, visual, kinesthetic, or analytical Attend to where the memory fails – add cue  Simply circling place in music calls attention to the point  visualize the circle

May not be conscious

 Mishra Jennifer

University of Houston

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

November, 2007

Williamon, A., & Valentine, E. (2002). The role of retrieval structures in memorizing music. Cognitive Psychology, 44(1), 1-32. Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

5

Memorization and the Performing Musician

As cognitive control of actions requires time, quick physical sequences must be automated to occur without conscious control. Far from dangerous, automating movements is necessary for a musical performance both for speed of movement and redirecting precious attention to interpretative rather than technical aspects of the music.



November, 2007

Any over-learned motor sequence requires automated muscle movements



 Fingers seem to move without conscious thought  Repeating motor movements automates process  Kinesthetic “memory” is conscious awareness

 Frees attention  Driving – automated series of movements allows attention to shift 

Confusion between kinesthetic “memory” and automating movements

of muscle movements

Attention is a bottle neck in human processing system – can only fully attend to one thing at a time



Much to attend to in performance  Automated to free attention  interpretation & communication with audience  Attend to demands of performance situation

 Must automate to perform complex

sequences of motor movements  Automated movements faster than conscious

thought

 Difficult sections 

Can’t generally verbalize automated procedures  Tie Shoes

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

Preparing for Performance    

Overlearning = Re-learning Repetition is ONE way of learning



Overlearning = Preparing for Performance  Not really memorizing

 superficial level of processing

 Confidence building

More connections = deeper processing = stronger memory Re-learn music Sensory “memories”

 Superstitious behaviors  Extra practice due to anxiety - especially when memorizing ▪ Performing without music increases anxiety ▪ Heightened anxiety results (sometimes) in poorer performance

 Aural  Visual  Kinesthetic 

Find additional patterns (analytical)

Craik, F. & Lockhart, R. (1972). Levels of processing: A framework for memory research. Journal of Verbal Learning & Verbal Behavior, 11, 671-684. Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

Leglar, M. A. (1978). Measurement of Indicators of anxiety levels under varying conditions of musical performance. Doctoral dissertation, Indiana University, 1978. November, 2007

Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

November, 2007

Part 3 Jennifer Mishra

University of Houston

Jennifer Mishra | [email protected]

November, 2007

6

Related Documents

Memory Wksp Ii
October 2019 3
Memory Wksp Iii
October 2019 8
Memory Wksp I
October 2019 11
Bmw Memory Options Ii
November 2019 9
In Memory Of Ii - Mirror
November 2019 14
Memory
November 2019 33