The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
The Impact of Interlingual Interference on Bilingual Brain: Slowdown in Word Processing in Hebrew as a Result of Auditory Similarity to Russian
Research Seminar on Visual Imagery Course (10547)
Student Name:
Igor Greenblat
Student ID Number:
308896638
Directed By:
Dr. Avner Caspi
1
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Contents
ABSTRACT ....................................................................................................................................................4
INTRODUCTION.............................................................................................................................................5 Attention ........................................................................................................................................................................ 5 Focused and Divided Attention .................................................................................................................................... 5 Task Automatization, Spatial orientation and Stroop Color-Word Task ................................................................ 6 Stroop Color-Word Test and its linkage to Attention flexibility ............................................................................... 6 Stroop Effect – General Research Review (McLeod, 1991)....................................................................................... 7 Emotional Stroop Effect (ESE) – a dispute regarding the nature of phenomena mechanism. Is ESE actually a Stroop task? The "Absence of Interference" argument by Daniel Algom (2005).................................................... 8 The Role of Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) in emotional and cognitive processing – a physiological support to the distinction between the Classic and Emotional Stroop Effects ....................................................................... 9 Bilingual Lexical Organization, Dependency/Interdependency between L1 and L2............................................. 10 Linking Bilinguality and Attention............................................................................................................................ 11 The Bilingual brain - Processing Levels and Cross-Lingual Interference.............................................................. 11 Taboo words as ESE stimuli....................................................................................................................................... 12 Emotional expressiveness of bilinguals in L1 and L2: Processing figurative language by bilinguals .................. 13 Dual Coding as an aggravating factor in Stroop task .............................................................................................. 13 Rationale of the current study.................................................................................................................................... 14 Independent Variables ................................................................................................................................................ 15 Dependent variable ..................................................................................................................................................... 15 Hypotheses ................................................................................................................................................................... 15
METHOD.......................................................................................................................................................17 Participants.................................................................................................................................................................. 17 Procedure ..................................................................................................................................................................... 17 Materials ...................................................................................................................................................................... 18
2
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
RESULTS .....................................................................................................................................................19 Data Preliminary Analysis.......................................................................................................................................... 19 ANOVA ........................................................................................................................................................................ 19 Descriptive Data .......................................................................................................................................................... 19
DISCUSSION ................................................................................................................................................21 Interpreting Findings.................................................................................................................................................. 21 Revising the Phenomenon of Interlingual Interference ........................................................................................... 21 Drawbacks and Artifacts of the Current Study; Suggestions for the Future Research ........................................ 23
REFERENCES..............................................................................................................................................27
APPENDIXES ...............................................................................................................................................34 Appendix I: Research group classification............................................................................................................... 34 Appendix II: Test stimuli sample (1x1 scaled) .......................................................................................................... 34 Appendix III: Task words list classification to groups............................................................................................. 35 Appendix IV: ANOVA Summary Table ................................................................................................................... 37 Appendix V: Group Means ........................................................................................................................................ 37 Appendix VI: Tukey HSD Test Values...................................................................................................................... 37 Appendix VII: Eta-Squared Data .............................................................................................................................. 37
3
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Abstract The impact of Reverse Interlingual Interference Effect (RI – word processing inhibition that occurs during translation from L2 to L1) on Russian-Hebrew bilingual's performance in Emotional Stroop task was examined. The difference in performance between bilinguals and monolinguals was tested with exactly the same sets of stimuli; when the stimuli was neutral (no auditory coherency between the word in Hebrew and any word in Russian lexicon) the difference in time responses was hypothesized to be a result of language "utilization seniority". Moreover, it was argued, that additionally to standard Stroop interference, bilingual participants' color naming is inhibited by unaware subliminal activation of semantic networks in Russian language (whether dominant or not) and to between-language competition due to auditory alikeness between the Stroop stimuli in Hebrew and words from Russian lexicon, rather than being an artifact of stimulus selection or experimental design. The hypotheses of the present study derive from theories and ascertainments in a field of cognitive psychology, such as Disturbed Feature Model (De Groot, 1992), Dual-Coding theory (Paivio, 1971), taboo words emotional impact on word processing (Siegrist, 1995), second language processing interference (Francis, 1999) and others. The findings suggest that auditory interference rooted in the similarity of the stimuli in L1 with obscene, concrete or abstract word is significantly incapable of activating alternative stimuli processing route and thus interfering with the cognitive processes involved in color-naming task. However, the observation of group means allows a certain level of speculation regarding the revealed tendencies.
4
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Introduction Attention The world we live in is full of sensory stimuli. If human brain had to process all the input entering the central nervous system, it probably would have collapsed under such an unbearable cognitive burden. Human brain data processing capabilities are much wider than animal's (Evans et al., 2005), although not infinite. In every given moment the cerebrum absorbs enormous amounts of data: sunlight, clock ticking, room temperature level and so on; different signals are processed differently due to various signal features as sharpness and intensity (Kutas & Federmeier, 1998). It is likely that monotonic serene chirp of a cricket will not interrupt our sleep thanks to habituation of our auditory system; in contrary a loud police siren noise will arouse us immediately (Condon & Weinberger, 1991). Is the way we respond to certain stimulus intentional and the decision to respond or to ignore various ambient occurrences is always taken consciously? More likely, the responses are extracted by our brain autonomously, without wasting our precious cognitive resources. Apparently, we owe our proper functioning to very complicated network of cognitive schemes "wise" enough to decide which of the concurrent input signals deserve high-level processing above the others. The cognitive mechanism responsible for input data filtering is Attention. Neisser (1974) defines it as "The assignation of the mechanisms of analysis to a limited part of the perceptual field". Focused and Divided Attention William James (1890) writes, "…Attention is taking possession by the mind, in clear and vivid form, of one out of what seem several simultaneously possible objects or trains of thought". The quote focuses in on the necessity of humans to take possession of only one object with their mind despite the several options present. The limited capacity of the human information processing system requires individuals to select the information they deem most important to attend to. Usually we focus and divide our limited attention resources according to the number of tasks we have to perform and the importance of those tasks (Kahneman, 1973; Navon & Gopher, 1979; Wickens, 1984). It is much easier to divide the attention voluntary between reading and chewing gum than between driving a car and speaking on a cell phone; in the last case we direct most of the attention resources to the speaking task and avert the attention from driving, which might cause an accident (Strayer et al., 2003). The
5
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
"neglected" tasks are sometimes performed automatically, without squandering attention resources. Automatization of a cognitive task requires experiencing; avoiding the route flags on the way down might be a routine task for an experienced skier, but not for an amateur. Attention can be attracted involuntarily, as if a lamp in a dark room suddenly lights up or voluntary, as if we want to observe "what will happen next", glazing at the traffic light. An "attention-drawing" stimulus is more salient than others; among those unique attributes are content subjective significance, such as hearing your own name (the "cocktail party effect", Cherry, 1953), background/foreground separation (white flower in a green field, Spieth, 1954; Webster, 1954) or emotional distinctiveness (snake in the grass; Anderson, 2005). Recent cognitive theories (Bunting & Cowans, 2005; Gopher, Greenshpan & Armoni, 1996) perceive the attention as a flexible resource, allocated accordingly to task requirements; we are able to focus the attention on relevant stimulus or divide it to various stimuli, if necessary. Task Automatization, Spatial orientation and Stroop Color-Word Task When a certain behavior is performed frequently and does not require intentional attention resources, it becomes automatic or "procedural" (Anderson, 1983). We perform automatic behaviors quickly, precisely and effortlessly, often involuntary, "it just happens". In order to explore the modus operandi of procedural behavior, psychological researchers simulate situations, where intentional and automatic responses conflict, the interference allows them to examine the latent features of the automatic behavior by testing their influence on observable behaviors. Such an influence perfectly outlined by Stroop Color-Word Test. In visual orientation, preattentive processes provide a spatial mapping of physical locations and allow us to target attention resources and to distinct the target from the background noise (Lamers & Roelofs, 2007). However, selecting a spatial location does not separate the target and the distractor in the Stroop task, since the word meaning and the color are spatially integrated or concentrated in a common part of a spatial field. Therefore, when attending the ink color of a Stroop-word stimulus, the meaning of a word receives attention resources as well and "sneaks" into our cognitive system. Stroop Color-Word Test and its linkage to Attention flexibility Stroop Effect, a "part of the limited golden fund of psychodiagnostic tools", as Emil Siska (2002) defines it, firstly presented in an article "Studies of Interference Serial Verbal
6
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Reactions", published in the Journal of Experimental Psychology in 1935. The effect is named after its inventor, John Ridley Stroop, who discovered it working on his PhD thesis in George Peabody College (today part of Vanderbilt University) and introduced the results to professional practice. When implementing the classic form of the Stroop color-word test, the subject is initially required to read words representing names of some basic colors, and quickly to name the ink color of these words, disregarding the actual meaning of the words. The phenomenon of interference, characterizing all the existing variations of Stroop test is caused by the competing phonological route of the graphical representation of the word (Tanenhaus, Flanihan and Seidenberg, 1980) and the actual color of the ink when the later is incongruent with the meaning of a word; this process is not intentional and unavoidable. The conflicting tasks involve focal attention to the critical element of the task which must be selected in competition with a dominant, semantic (phonologic) element. However, word naming is not affected by the color of the ink (MacLeod, 1991) due to the prevalence of phonology over physical characteristics of the stimuli. In order to perform in Stroop Task, the participant should obtain some flexibility implicating attention resources; neuroimaging of adults who suffer from Attention Deficit Disorder (ADD), unlike the normal controls show no activation of the Anterior Cingulated area of the cortex, involved in regulation of cognitive activity (Bush et al., 1998). Instead, they show greater activity on incompatible trials in the Anterior Insula (Bush et al., 1999). As was suggested in the study of word association, the Insula represents a more automatic pathway than the Anterior Cingulate, ADD patients having difficulties to dissipate attention efficiently and let it be attracted subliminally, instead of targeting intentionally. Stroop Effect – General Research Review (McLeod, 1991) In his monumental taxonomy "Half a Century of Research in Stroop Effect, an Integrative Review", Colin M. MacLeod (1991) covered the history of the attempt to combine the word and color stimuli in a potentially conflicting situation. Since the original experiment by J. R. Stroop, the number of published articles on issue exceeded 700, probably the investigators were appealed by the simplicity and the reliability of the experiment. Various versions of the task appeared, including the picture-word interference task (Dyer, 1973), auditory task (Hamers, 1973), sorting and matching task (Tesse & Happ, 1964).
7
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Emotional Stroop Effect (ESE) – a dispute regarding the nature of phenomena mechanism. Is ESE actually a Stroop task? The "Absence of Interference" argument by Daniel Algom (2005) It has been shown that emotion is an important element in directing attention in the working memory processes (Mather et al., 1995). In "Emotional Stroop Paradigm" participants are asked to name the color of a printed words partly loaded with emotional connotation. Apparently, naming a color of an unpleasant word such as "grief", "fear" or "death" takes more time comparatively to neutral words (Williams, Mathews & MacLeod, 1996; Sharma & McKenna, 2001; Whalen et al., 1998). The "Emotional Stroop Paradigm" is a very popular tool not solely in the cognitive research area, but in clinical-psychopathological area as well (investigating eating disorders - Dobson & Dozois, 2001; alcohol dependency – Flannery et al., 2007; PTSD patients – Emilien et al., 2000; schizophrenia - Henik et al., 2002; and also Constatntine, McNally & Hornig, 2001; Kindt & Brosschot, 1997; Lavy & van der Hout, 1993; Watts, McKenna, Sharrock & Tresize, 1986). The classic approach to the explanation of the phenomenon is that additional attention resources required while processing emotionally negative stimuli since the usual color naming response interferes with the subliminal processing of the emotional stimuli (Freyd, 1996; Williams, Mathews & McLeod, 1996). However, not all the researchers agree that ESE is an attention (or lack of attention) phenomenon. A group of psychologists from Tel-Aviv university claims, that emotionally loaded stimuli is processed slower because of the activation of "general-purpose defense mechanism" that responds to a threat by "temporarily slowing down or even freezing all the ongoing activity" (Lev, 2002; Algom, Ben David & Levy, 2003). Since a major slowdown for emotional words has been found in studies of lexical decision and reading aloud as well, the researchers reject the claim that ESE caused by cognitive overload or incapability of the selective attention mechanism; furthermore, Algom even rejects the argument that ESE is a subcategory of Stroop tests array. He claims that ESE lacks the essential features of a Stroop task – the Item-Specific Interference and Congruent Conditions. For the color Stroop the interference can be calculated at the level of the item and not at the level of the whole list, as it is in the ESE. Moreover, there's no congruent condition in ESE. The semantic conflict (in the incongruent condition) and agreement (in congruent condition) that forms the basis of the color Stroop effect is absent from the ESE phenomenon. In this sense, the Emotional Stroop task is not really a Stroop task at all.
8
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
A group of University of California researchers (Mackay et al., 2004) reject the classic "attention-related" perception as well, defining the described inhibition phenomena as an "Inhibitory mechanism preventing awareness of taboo words" and explaining it by "enhancement of the explicit memory during encoding". Klauer (2003) proposes an additional explanation to ESE phenomenon – "Automatic Vigilance", an unaware evolutional readiness to give more attention to processing of threatening stimuli. Automatic vigilance occurs when a negatively valence target stimulus (an image of a COCKROACH) is categorized more accurately when it is preceded by a threatening prime stimulus (e.g., the word DISEASE) than a neutral prime stimulus (Hermans, DeHouwer, & Eelen, 2001). Similarly to a reflex, it happens without our awareness or effort, and runs to completion without conscious monitoring. The effects may be far-reaching, especially when automatic vigilance impacts on cognitive resources such as attention and memory. However, the Automatic Vigilance Theory supporters can't explain the inhibition in ESE when there's no priming condition; ESE researches show extended response times even when the participant processes the emotional stimuli in the first time, without any preliminary priming. All recent hypotheses concentrate on mechanisms that differ from the classic theory: the evolutionary coping explanation (Algom et al. version), memory phenomena (in Mackay et al. research) and Automatic Vigilance version (Klauer et al.) considered to be in the focus of ESE, and not the attention. The Role of Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) in emotional and cognitive processing – a physiological support to the distinction between the Classic and Emotional Stroop Effects The dissociation between the classical Stroop task and ESE was proven empirically in both lab experiments (Algom, Chajut & Lev, 2004) and clinical experience of abused PostTraumatic Stress Disorder patients, who's symptoms were dramatically facilitated during ESE performing and remained almost unaffected during the classical Stroop (Bremner et al., 2004). Recent theoretical and experimental work (Botvinick at al., 2001; Drevets and Raichle 1998, Bush et al, 2000) has shown that Anterior Cingulate Cortex (ACC) may be divided into two parts, each half exclusively responsible for different type of mental processing. When strong emotions are involved in a Stroop test or any other task, the dorsal area of ACC is less active than at rest, while cognitive conflict tasks, such as the classic Stroop task suppress
9
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
activity in the ventral ACC. fMRI evidence from a variety of tasks indicates that two parts might be mutually inhibitory – cognitive tasks knocking down the emotional activity in ACC and vice versa. According to the presented findings, emotional words amplify mental activity in the emotional "half" of the ACC and depress the processing in the second "half" which controls the cognitive processing. Research of the emotional Stroop involving both control (George et al., 1994; Whalen et al., 1998) and clinical (Rauch et al., 1994; Rauch et al., 1996) population have shown high levels of activation in both ventral and dorsal ACC, when traditional Stroop studies (Bush et al., 1998; Carter, Mintun & Cohen, 1995) have shown activities in caudal part of ACC. The described neuropsychological dissociation is leading to the conclusion that two tasks (the classical Stroop and ESE) are distinct since the anatomical modules being activated during the tasks are different and even mutually inhibitory. On the other hand, the notion that emotion and cognition are functionally interdependent is supported at both the behavioral and neuroanatomical level. When faced with stimuli or situations that elicit negative affect (ESE, for example), people rely on regulatory processes mainly involved in attention (Rothbart, Posner & Hershey, 1995). PET and MRI studies have linked both the traditional and emotional Stroop to a network of neural systems critical for the expression and self-regulation of emotion (van Honk et al., 2000; West & Alain, 2000). Those observations do not contradict the previous statement concerning the distinction of the Classic and the Emotional Stroop tasks, but only emphasize the complexity of the human mind and the interdependence of its processes. Bilingual Lexical Organization, Dependency/Interdependency between L1 and L2 Over fifty percents of world population speaks at least two languages. Posing unique challenges to the human brain, multilingualism requires simultaneous activation of two or more sets of rules, sometimes very different ones. A bilingual individual, in order to escape the curse of Babel, has to juggle between two languages performing code switching (a rapid switching from one language to another, Dehaene, 1999); avoiding the cross-talk apparently requires operating of sophisticated mechanisms of segregation and coordination (Dehaene, 1999). In one of the physiological researches (Price at al., 1999) scientists used Positron Emission Tomography techniques (PET) in order to clarify the "modus operandi" of a bilingual brain. Unsurprisingly, switching between two or more languages and translating from one language to another requires activation of additional language brain areas
10
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
additionally to the "classical" ones (such as Broca's Area). Seems, that bilingual brain, at least among highly proficient bilinguals, addresses the problems of lexical data processing by operating with a unique plasticity; the pattern of bilingual cerebral activation is different for each language and some brain regions are activated in one language but not in other (Perani et al., 1998). Do bilinguals use two languages independently, alternating between them, or keep both languages activated simultaneously and process every stimulus in two parallel lexical routes? This question is investigated widely in psychocognitive research, the interaction of lexical processing between the first (L1) and the second languages (L2) argued to be independent or interdependent. A traditional language switch hypotheses (Gerard & Scarborough, 1989; MacNamara & Kushnir, 1971) assume independent selective activation and deactivation of the languages, while parallel activation theories (Grainger & Dijkstra, 1992; Chen & Ho, 1986; Cummins, 2000) challenge the traditional ones, claiming that robust competition exist between and within language effects in both languages. Linking Bilinguality and Attention As one item is selected to be attended to, the individual must inhibit the competing items. Inhibition is therefore a critical feature of attentional control. Both deal with the concept that many tasks require fixed attention to certain aspects of the problem while ignoring the other. Inhibition is used daily, for example, when choosing groceries in the supermarket. A variety of milk cartons are available and one must inhibit the motor act of ignoring the undesired cartons on the shelf so that the chosen carton can be simply picked. Bilinguals are constantly required to use this skill when speaking and listening to one language and ignoring the other. The Bilingual brain - Processing Levels and Cross-Lingual Interference How do speakers of more than one language represent and process the words in each language? Kroll and Stewart (1994) claim that word forms associated with the dominant and non-dominant languages are "located" in two independent "word banks", the Revised Hierarchical Model (RHM) proposes distinct lexical word form representations in each language, but a common conceptual system. A similar "Bilingual Production" model presented by international group of researchers (Costa, Colome & Caramazza, 2000; Illes et al., 1999) claims that a common single semantic representation level for both L1 and L2 co-
11
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
exists in parallel with distinct lexical representations for each language. Some studies have reported evidence suggesting that both alternatives are possible (Champagnol, 1975; Dyer, 1971; Durgunoglu & Roediger III, 1987; Kolers, 1963; Preston & Lambert, 1969), implying that the issue of single versus dual memory storage in bilinguals may be a matter of interpretation. How do two distinct lexicons co-exist and interact in bilingual brain? La Heij et al. (1996) claim, that the semantic context has a more powerful effect on the processing in the backward direction than in the forward direction (L2-L1 versus L1-L2 translation). Ambiguous words or words with multiple translations are processed differently and more slowly comparatively to concrete words. De Groot (1992) explains in Disturbed Feature Model that concrete words share the same representational distribution across languages, they are more likely to overlap in meaning and therefore being translated more quickly. In this model, semantic concepts are not represented by single nodes, but by a bundle of feature nodes. Each word activates a pattern of features in both languages. Most of the researchers share the agreement that processing within the language always interferes with the processing between the languages. When a bilingual participant tries to name the color of the word in L2, the Stroop interference (top-down processes superiority) causes a subliminal processing of the word, even though the later is not required and slowing down the response time (Francis, 1999). Taboo words as ESE stimuli Since obscene words are taboo in the modern society, they appear as "emotionally loaded" stimuli and therefore fit the definition of the Emotional Stroop task stimuli. Word forms can directly activate emotions: when people name the color of randomly intermixed taboo and neutral words, color naming times are longer for taboo than for neutral words (Siegrist, 1995); this suggestion presents a theoretical base for the hypothesis of the current research. For monolingual speakers, recall and recognition tests are influenced similarly by emotionality, with emotion words showing an advantage compared to neutral words (Rubin & Friendly, 1986). Jay (2000) defines obscene words as "super emotion" in terms of the diversity and strength of associated contexts and emotions. When scientists measured the electrodermal activity (EDA) of participants exposed to swear and neutral words (Bowels & Pleydell-Pearce, 2004), autonomic response to swear words was indubitably longer than to neutral stimuli. Taboo words activate the amygdala (LaBar & Phelps, 1998), known to be a
12
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
key subcortical structure for threat-detection. MacKay et al. (2002) argued that the superiority of recall for taboo words occurs because emotional reactions during encoding facilitate binding of the taboo word to its context. Emotional expressiveness of bilinguals in L1 and L2: Processing figurative language by bilinguals Diverse studies support the idea that the first language is bilingual's choice for expressing emotions (Javier, Barroso & Munos, 2003; Sechrst, Flores & Arellano, 1968; Anooshian & Hertel, 1994; Bloom & Beckwith, 1989). If the first language is linked to emotional expressiveness, the second language may be the language of emotional indifference: people discuss taboo issues (Bong & Lai, 1986), tend to reveal more secrets (Gonzales & Reigosa, 1976) and report less anxiety to obscene and taboo words (Gonzales & Reigosa, 1976) in L2. There are few studies focusing on the figurative aspect of language comprehension and production (Francis, 1996; Souto Silva, 2000), but even fewer studies covered the obscene language aspects. However, an existing wide research base of the emotional aspect of bilingual cognition allows us to predict certain response patterns of bilingual participants to taboo (obscene) stimuli in L1 or auditory similar stimuli in L2 which activates the obscene network in L1. I hypothesize, that neutral word in L2 auditory similar to obscene language stimuli (swear is a part of a figurative language) in L1 will interfere the usual processing of the word in L2 among bilinguals. Actually, any auditory similarity between the target word in L2 and any word in L1 should interfere the usual processing in L2 as was hypothesized before, but additionally to the auditory similarity the obscene word as a taboo-emotionallyloaded stimuli might cause an additional inhibition in mental processing. Dual Coding as an aggravating factor in Stroop task Cognition according to Dual-Coding Theory (DCT, proposed by Allan Paivio in 1971 and developed in 1986) involves the activity of two different subsystems; a verbal system specialized for dealing with language and imagery system, dedicated to pictorial processing. Both systems are involved in language processing and the interplay between them depends on the developmental level and on the nature of the stimuli – difficult to image abstract words will be processed mainly verbally and concrete words both verbally and visually. A concrete word that can be imagined as a pictorial representation of its semantic meaning is easier to
13
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
process; the coding into Long Term Memory is performed on the double basis - verbal and visual. Memory footprints of concrete words are therefore stronger comparatively to abstract words, the coding and the decoding (recollection) of the word is faster and easier. The expected additive memory benefit of dual code has been confirmed in numerous experiments (Paivio, 1975; Paivio & Lambert, 1981). In 1986 Paivio presented the Bilingual Dual-Coding Theory (BDCT). The BDCT proposes an architecture in which a common imagery system is connected to two verbal systems which are linked to each other via associative connections. The interconnection between the three systems explains the interdependent functional behavior. Keeping in mind the reinforced coding of the "imaginable" word, I hypothesize that Bilinguals will process Stroop Task words in L2 (Hebrew) auditory similar to concrete words in L1 (Russian) longer than the abstract words, the color naming response will be inhibited because of the subliminal processing of the pictorial representation (an image) additionally to concept processing of the printed word. For example, a word "soroka" ("crow" in Russian), according to Dual-Coding Theory, will activate both pictorial and verbal networks while processing the auditory similar Hebrew word "sruka" (the Hebrew alphabet allows both words' similar typing), participant's imagination will draw a picture of a craw, additionally to the verbal stimuli. However, processing the Hebrew word "ot" ("from" in Russian) will activate the verbal network of the coherent word solely, since abstract words are impossible to visualize. Words in Hebrew with an auditory similarity to concrete words in Russian will not have any effect on Monolingual Hebrew speakers since no semantic networks will be activated. Rationale of the current study Bilinguals seem always to activate L1 while processing words, but the results about whether bilinguals activate L2 when they are attending to L1 are conflicting (Marian, Spivey, & Hirsch, 2003; Xue et al., 2004). In order to cause bilinguals parallel activation of both languages while processing a stimuli word in Stroop color task, the presented words should be unique (attention-drawing, based on Cocktail party effect, Cherry, 1953). Three "unique" auditory connections of the presented stimuli were chosen for the current work; it was either concrete, obscene (taboo) or abstract. Each of sixty participants, partly Israeli-born without any knowledge in Russian language, partly bilingual immigrants whose primary language (L1) is Russian and secondary (L2)
14
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Hebrew or vice versa (L1- Hebrew and L2-Russian) was asked to name as quickly and precisely as possible the color of printed words in Hebrew presented on the screen of notebook computer. The response times were recorded using Direct RT software and processed. Hebrew words selected for the task were classified into four groups: neutral (no particular auditory linkage to any word in Russian), auditory coherent to abstract, concrete and obscene words in Russian. None of the auditory Hebrew-Russian similar word categories was predicted to affect the response times of monolingual participants above the average response time for a neutral stimuli. Independent Variables Group type – three values were defined for this variable: Monolingual, Bilingual (speaking both Hebrew and Russian) with Hebrew as a dominant language and Russian-dominant Bilingual. Stroop Stimuli type or Reverse Interference type – there were four conditions: the neutral condition – no auditory overlap between the Hebrew stimuli and any word in Russian, an auditory coherence with a abstract word (Top-Down Interference only), an auditory coherence with a concrete word (Top-Down Interference + Dual Coding impact) and an auditory coherence with a obscene word (Top-Down Interference + Emotional taboo-related effect) in Russian. Dependent variable Response Time – since it is usually impossible to monitor cognitive processes directly, a collateral way of dependent variable monitoring – the "footprints inquiry" will be utilized. Using response times measurement as an indicator to the complexity of a cognitive process is a usual technique in the field of cognitive psychology. It's more than reasonable to assume, that longer response time represents more complex mental process. Hypotheses The current research consists of 3x4 array; three groups (one control group of monolinguals and two test groups comprised of bilinguals with Hebrew or Russian as dominant language) participated in an experiment, while three lexical interference conditions were applied (no
15
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
interference means that the Hebrew stimuli is neutral, that is to say that it's not auditory coherent with any word in Russian, abstract, concrete, or obscene stimuli). For the monolingual group the response times (color naming times) are not expected to vary accordingly to auditory similarity of any kind between task words in Hebrew (their native language. It was hypothesized, that the performance of both test groups will be influenced by interference caused by auditory coherence of the processed word in Hebrew to abstract, concrete or obscene word in Russian language. Since the coherency to concrete words supposedly is influenced by Dual Coding and the color naming is inhibited by Reverse Interference, both semantically and visually, the color naming slowdown in this case is hypothesized to be stronger (longer response times) relatively to abstract words condition. It was also expected, that moderate slowdown among Hebrew-Dominant Bilinguals in all the experimental conditions comparatively to Russian-Dominant Bilinguals will occur; they lack the theoretical-semantic aspects of language acquisition and the knowledge of obscene lexicon. It is logical to assume that Russian-dominant bilingual subjects were quite comfortable speaking Hebrew for most of the day, and going home and conversing with their parents, grandparents, and siblings in Russian. However, their results are predicted to be slower than Monolinguals' since their knowledge of Russian language is beyond zero.
16
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Method Participants The sample was comprised of sixty adult male and female participants (ages ranged from 22 to 46, with mean of 27.8 and SD of 7.6) drawn from heterogenic population – peers (university students and friends) and colleagues. The bilingual participants were children of immigrants from the former Soviet Union, either Israeli-born or born in USSR, all of them acquired their Hebrew skills while being in Israel; fluent in Hebrew and at least at the basic level in Russian (based upon self-report). The initial identification for Hebrew-dominant bilinguals was "Russian-speakers born in Israel", however due to inability to spotter the relevant population the criterion was "softened" : the participants who immigrated to Israel before the age of six or reported "Poor" Russian level were classified as "Bilinguals with Hebrew as a dominant language"; those who immigrated after the age of six and reported at least a "Reasonable" proficiency level were classified as "Bilinguals with Russian as a dominant language". Israeli-born participants were classified as "Monolinguals", unless identified their Russian speaking proficiency level as "Reasonable" (two participants). The control group was comprised of Israeli-born participants, fluent in Hebrew and completely non-proficient in Russian. Procedure All the participants were asked (in Hebrew) if they wish to participate in an anonymous research voluntary, those who agreed provided the following information: 1. Number of years speaking Russian solely before obtaining any knowledge in Hebrew (if applicable). 2. Age when immigrated to Israel (if applicable). 3. Speaking proficiency level in Russian: Poor, Reasonable or Good (if applicable). Upon completion of the questionnaire the participants were tested using the Emotional Stroop Task. Subjects were tested individually in similar conditions, during their leisure time (no pressure of any kind was observed). The participants were instructed to ignore the meaning of the words and to respond as quickly and precisely as possible to the color of the presented stimuli by pressing either the red button located in the left side of notepad keyboard or the green button located in the right part, in accordance with the color of the presented word. The
17
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
participants were told that each word would be presented by the software on a black screen and will be replaced by another word upon pressing one of the buttons. The pattern was held constant – thirty four words were presented accordingly to pre-set list in a constant order; words of all categories were mixed (the list is presented in "Appendix III" section). The participants performed a "pilot" adjustment test comprised of five stimuli words before the actual task; the results of the pilot test were not processed. Three categories contained what was hypothesized as "potentially sensitive stimuli" - abstract, concrete or obscene words (the "sensitive" linkage was auditory, between the target word in Hebrew and words from Russian lexicon). One category (neutral stimuli) was selected to act as a control condition. Each word was approximately matched for length and syllables. After completion of the task the participants were debriefed. The response times to all the stimuli were monitored, filed and processed. It was assumed, that bilingual participants are familiar with the obscene Russian slang and monolinguals are not; the assumption was verified after the completion of the test. Results of participants who did not meet the criteria (there were six of them) were disqualified. Materials The test stimuli were generated using Direct RT software, the program allows response time calculation as well. Direct RT software is designed by Empirisoft Company (the shareware version limited to three weeks is downloadable from internet site www.empirisoft.com.) Stimuli were presented on 14" notepad PC monitor in Times New Roman font, with letter size of 140. The colors were easily extinguished (saturated Green and Red).
18
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Results Data Preliminary Analysis Due to extraordinary length (more or less than two syllables), in order to sustain the equability of the stimuli, four words were eliminated from the initial list. Wrong responses (1.44% of overall quantity) and answers of extraordinarily long or short response times (Mean +/- 2 Standard Deviations) were excluded from the final list as well, 343 out of overall 1800 responses were not included in the final list (19.06%). It is important to note, that 188 out of 420 responses to abstract stimuli were eliminated; the validity of "abstract stimulirelated" results therefore is questionable. ANOVA A 4x3 factorial ANOVA showed a significant main effect of participant's group type on the response time , F (2,228) =3.19, p < .05, although no significant main effect of Stimuli type F (3,228) =0.295, p > .05. The interaction between variables was not significant, F (6,228) =0.24, p > .05. The relevant statistical data is presented in "Appendix IV– ANOVA Table". Descriptive Data Interference in Word Processing
480 Bilinguals L1 Hebrew
460
Bilinguals L1 Russian
440 420 400
Monolinguals (Hebrew Only)
e
ra l ut Ne
re t nc
Co
tr a Ab s
Ta bo
o
ct
380
Response Times (microseconds)
500
Stimuli Type
"Appendix V" presents means for the three groups in the four conditions. The partial parallelism of the curves (no intersection) is coherent with the failure to reject the null hypothesis for interaction test between the Group Type and the Stimuli Type.
19
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Post-hoc analysis In average, bilinguals responded significantly slower by 8.36% than monolinguals to all stimuli categories (demonstrating significant main effect of Group Type variable): neutral, abstract, concrete and taboo. The response times in concrete and taboo conditions differed significantly (11.05% and 10.53% respectively). In neutral condition the responses of bilinguals were "delayed" by 5.72% and in abstract condition by 2.47%, both measures insignificant. Tukey Post-Hoc test provided no additional insights, while the minimal rejection value for relevant Dfwithin and k levels (228 and 4 respectively, α=0.05) is 3.63, the maximal Mx vs My Tukey's HSD test values between the means (drawn from the means table in "Appendix V") was below 1.5 and therefore none of the differences between the test conditions (stimuli type) was statistically significant. The HSD values are presented in "Appendix VI". Chi-Square test revealed that more than 96% of the total variance is attributable to Error, remaining therefore less than 4% to the main and interaction effects, while the only significant effect (the group effect) accounts for 2.6% of the variance score only. The following chart is representing the percentage of each effect's contribution to the total variance. "Appendix VII" contains the relevant statistical data.
Relative Effect Sizes (Eta Squared) Group Type, 0.026960214 Stimuli Type, 0.003740854
Group Type
Group - Stimuli Interaction , 0.006176044
Stimuli Type Group - Stimuli Interaction Error
Error, 0.963122888
20
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Discussion Interpreting Findings Knowing an additional language slows down the response times in Stroop Color Words task (significant main effect of Group Type variable). However, seems like the inhibition in processing is not caused by parallel meaning interference. Namely, neither Hebrew-dominant nor Russian-dominant bilingual participants did not necessarily activate Russian language lexicon when the stimuli words in Hebrew were auditory similar to concrete, abstract or obscene words in Russian. Keeping in mind that Russian-dominant bilinguals generally performed slower than Hebrew-dominant bilinguals and both bilingual groups performed slower than monolinguals, appears that the difference in language proficiency is the only meaningful reason for the inhibition (Russian-dominants have probably less experience utilizing Hebrew than Hebrew-dominants and certainly than Hebrew monolinguals). No significant main effects of Stimuli type and interaction were observed. However, combining the perceivable (but not significant) disparities between group means and the validation of significant main effect of Group Type allows to claim for a certain coherency of the results with the hypothesized phenomena: time gaps between the responses of monolingual-bilingual and Hebrew dominant –Russian dominant participant groups were larger in concrete and taboo than in neutral conditions (the results of the abstract cluster are questionable due to the multiple exclusion of responses). It can be speculated, that the explanation of the differences lies in the hypothesized second-language interferences and not in Hebrew proficiency level solely; we would have expected similar differences in all stimuli categories if the their only cause was "language utilizing experience". Revising the Phenomenon of Interlingual Interference Because the field of bilingualism is still relatively new, studies in the linguistics, psycholinguistics, language development and neurolinguistics of bilingualism have often produced conflicting results; the findings of the current work (the absence of main stimuli type and interaction effects) were analyzed via different prisms, each representing a distinct perception of the issue. According to Dual-Coding Theory (Paivio & Desrochers, 1980; Paivio & Lambert, 1981), the imagery system and the two verbal systems corresponding to each language of the bilingual are independent although partly interconnected. The independency explains the ability of
21
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
bilinguals to encode, store and retrieve non-verbal objects (images) without any interventions from either of the verbal systems. Seems, that stimuli type effect didn't appear because the participants of the current research treated the presented stimuli as images, which allowed them to avoid the activation of verbal networks upon the recognition of the color. In order to verify the hypothesis, an alternative (auditory) form of stimuli representation might be used. Grosjean (1999) claims, that the core issue in bilinguals' communication is the language mode his mind is set-up. Language mode is "the state of activation of the bilingual’s languages and language processing mechanisms at a given point in time" and it depends on the environmental context; among bilinguals the preferred activation language of a monolingual will be L1, while among his L2 monolinguals the focus will be on L2. Since the test was presented in Hebrew, including the performance instructions and the language of the application, the participants "switched" their brains to "Hebrew mode" priory to the performance of the task and therefore did not recognize the auditory hints as Russian words. In order to verify the assumption, an additional research with test instructions in L1 for bilingual participants is required. If the two languages of a bilingual share the same script, a visually presented stimulus from either language has been found to activate words from both languages. Clear evidence of that can be found in a research on the non-selective nature of lexical access (Altenberg and Cairns, 1983; Dijkstra, Van Jaarsveld, & Ten Brinke, 1998; Nas, 1983). In this case, when there is a language switch, bilinguals do not have to deactivate one lexicon and activate the other because both lexicons are simultaneously active. In the current research, the languages distinct orthographically and the activation of another (Russian) script is not automatic; it might require an additional processing time. However, once the appropriate lexicon has been activated, no extra time should be required during the verification process because the only set of lexical representations available for examination belongs to the target language (Kirsner et al., 1984) and therefore semantic networks in Russian remain "activated" and the response to the color doesn’t involve additional cognitive processing of the word in a parallel language. Since no significant time differences observed between monolinguals and bilinguals as a response to the first word (the word "krova" was expected to be easily auditory recognizable as a concrete word in Russian "korova" – "a cow" causing bilinguals to "switch" the language mode from Hebrew to Russian) – apparently no "switching procedure" was
22
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
performed by Russian-speakers. Namely, they didn't notice the secondary meaning of the presented stimuli at all, which explains the absence of the stimuli type main effect. Recent work on the emotional Stroop paradigm (McKenna & Sharma 2004) has provided evidence that using negatively valenced emotional words instead of color words has a slowing effect primarily on the subsequent trial. The results of the current research did not demonstrate a significant emotional Stroop-related slowdown since the stimuli did not activate the relevant verbal networks in the minds of bilingual participants. The ESE slowdown, whether it is based on attention deficit or on threatening effect (Algom, 2002) was not observed because it was not initiated; the existence of the effect therefore remains unclear. The current research clearly has the drawback of the way the stimuli is presented; it fails to penetrate the automaticity of the human brain. The significant main effect of Group Type was predicted to occur due to the phenomenon of "Reverse" or "Inverse" translation (Brysbaert & Duyck, 2004). Bilingual participants translate the presented stimuli from Hebrew to Russian in order to process it; therefore their response times are slower. It is probable that the same reason of the Group Type effect existence is "guilty" for the absence of the Stimuli Type effect: while being occupied with reverse translation, bilinguals fail to recognize the auditory alikeness of the stimuli with words in Russian. Drawbacks and Artifacts of the Current Study; Suggestions for the Future Research The phonological complexity of a word of stimuli words, if not controlled, might be an artifact and can severely damage the reliability of the research; therefore the "word-structure" parameter was added to the word list to cope with the problem and some of the initially prepared words were removed from the final list. Some researchers investigated the "wordlength effect" (Baddeley et al., 1975; Schweickert & Boruff, 1986) and found, that sequences with shorter durations were remembered better than sequences with longer durations. Longer words probably will require longer procession times; therefore we should expect extended response times when the stimulus is longer. In the present study equal was the only way to control the stimuli equality, however equalizing the amount of syllables does not always even the length of the words. The second possible "underwater rock" that might affect the reliability of the research is the personal language ability of the participants or, in other words, the internal variance within the test groups or the level of "bilingual balance" (Brysbaert & Duyck, 2004). A group of 23
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Panteton University researchers (Archonti, Protopapas & Skaloumbakas, 2006) suggest, that greater Stroop interference was observed in children diagnosed with reading disability (dyslexia) than in unimpaired children, moreover, poorer reading skills were found to correlate with greater Stroop interference in the general school population. The distribution of monolingual population to test groups was not sensitive enough to the personal language abilities, although the participants' questionnaire, including a question according his fluency (based on self-report) in Russian might provide some valuable knowledge. The degree of balance or fluency in bilinguals is always a delicate matter. Some researchers have concluded that being equally proficient in both one’s languages is more of a cognitive ideal than reality (Hakuta, Ferdman, & Diaz, 1987). Since there is no widely accepted method of assessing bilingual proficiency, self reports are quite common in bilingual research. In the present experiment, self reports were also used, by means of a language questionnaire that was given to subjects. The cases where a second language is acquired late, but comes to be the dominant language were not inquired, this can happen when one immigrates and marries a native speaker of the L2, and raises children whose dominant language is the L2 (Pavlenko, 2004). It also would be interesting to test the words that the bilingual participants remembered, Corkin & Kensinger (2003) claimed, that emotional words are more vividly remembered than the neutral, perhaps the automatic responses prevented immediate activation of the parallel language, but were remembered after all (McKay et al, 2004, Siegrist, 1995), although tacitly presented. Bilinguality is influenced by many variables, such as socioeconomic status, cognitive development (skills), personality, sociolinguistic proficiency and motivation (Romaine, 1995). None of these variables were measured in the current research. The motivation factor is particularly important; bilinguals "switch" languages depending on social situation. A bilingual friend, a manager in a high-tech company has told me that he doesn't speak Russian with his Russian-speaking engineers because they simply ignore his instructions, probably feeling that speaking the same "second" language equalizes people even in a manager-worker conditions. While some of the Russian-speaking Israelis make efforts towards a full integration into the Israeli society, denying any Russian roots, other separate themselves from the Israeli culture, feeling more Russian than Israeli. The tendency towards integration/separation should be controlled.
24
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
It was predicted that between-language competition would be observed from Russian to Hebrew. But is the interference process symmetrical across languages? Combining a list of stimuli in Russian with auditory coherency to Hebrew words was considered initially but eventually turned out to be too complicated; a satisfactory amount of such corresponding words was not found. An additional problem was locating a control group – Russian speakers with no proficiency in Hebrew. A "reversed" experiment is required in order to conclude about the bidirectionality of the measured effect. The word convergence (hyperonym) problem is another possible obstacle. The meaning of a word consists of a list of conceptual features, comparable to what one finds in a dictionary (Levelt, 1989; Roelof, 1992, 1996). For example, the Russian word SLIL (слил) may be represented by two conceptual features PEED or FLUSHED. If the Russian-speaking participant wants to express a message containing the concept of the mentioned word, it is unclear which of the conceptual features PEED or FLUSHED will be activated. The problem is acute since the classification of the stimuli, either obscene (body excretions considered obscene) or neutral depends on the participant's interpretation of the word, performed upon the presentation of the Hebrew stimuli with the same sound ( )סלילmeaning COIL. In many cases the underlying conceptual representation of the word is so rich and complex that it can only be expressed by using that particular word. The relative frequency of the stimuli in both languages was not controlled. Some of the words might be obsolete or unfamiliar to majority of Russian-speaking audience. A careful study in which word frequency is an independent variable may prove insightful. The gender data of the participants was not collected. Although, there is a possibility that the gender has its effect on the responses in Emotional Stroop task, as Thomson & Platek (2007) demonstrated, males are probably more affected by sexual jealousy priming while performed jealousy-based Stroop task and therefore might experience higher emotional interference as a auditory coherency of L1 stimuli to sexual-jealousy related obscene words in L2. The co-existence of multiple languages in one brain suggests that sophisticated mechanisms of segregation and coordination must exist in order to prevent the cross-talk between the languages (Dehaene, 1999). By improving the comprehension of bilingual processing of "double-lingual" stimuli, researchers might contribute to practical knowledge of brain plasticity and critical learning processes.
25
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Is one's conceptual system is changing upon learning another languages? Namely, are Russian-Hebrew bilinguals more "Russian" or more "Hebrew"? Wolff & Ventura (2003) claim that Russian-English bilinguals who had learned English patterned in their condition conceptualizing as English monolinguals, even though they did the experimental task in Russian. Seems, like learning a second language has its consequences for the underlying conceptual system, it can change the way one views the world.
26
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
References Abrams, L., Dyer, J.R., MacKay, D.G., Marian, D.E., Shafto, M., & Taylor, J.K. (2004). Relations between emotion, memory and attention: evidence from taboo stroop, lexical decision and immediate memory tasks. Memory and Cognition 32 (3), 474-488. Afzal, N., Bremner, J.D., Charney, D.S., Elzinga, B., Schmahl, C., Vermetten, E., & Vythilingam. (2004). M. Neural correlates of the classic Color and Emotional Stroop in women with Abuse-Related Post Traumatic Stress Disorder. Biological Psychiatry, 55, 612620. Algom, D., Ben David, B., & Levy, L. (2003). The Emotional Stroop Effect as a generic reaction to threat, not a selective reaction to specific semantic categories. In Berglund, B. & Borg, E. (Eds.), Fechner Day 2003, 21-25. Stockholm: International Society for Psychophysics. Alvarado, J.S., Christman, J.C., Freyd, J.J., Hayes, A.E., & Martorello, S.R. (1998). Cognitive environments and dissociative tendencies: Performance on the standard Stroop task for high versus low dissociators. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 12, S91-S103. Alvarez, C.J., Carreiras, M., & de Vega, M. (2000). Syllable-frequency effect in visual word recognition: Evidence of sequential-type processing. Psicologica, 21, 341-374. Alvarez, R.P., Grainger, J., & Holcomb, P.J. (2003). Accessing word meaning in two languages: an event-related brain potential study of beginning bilinguals. Brain and Language, 87, 290-304. Anderson, J.R. (1983). The architecture of cognition. Cambridge, MA: Harward University Press. Archonti, A., Protopapas, A., & Skaloumbakas, C. (2006). Reading ability is negatively related to Stroop interference. In press. (Cognitive psychology).
27
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Armoni, L., Greenshpan, Y., & Gopher, D. (1996). Switching attention between tasks: Exploration of the components of executive control and their development with training. In proceeding of the Human Factors and Ergonomics Society 40th Annual Meeting, 1060-1064. Philadelphia: HFES. Aycicegi, A., Gleason, J.B., & Harris, C.L. (2003). Taboo words and reprimands elicit greater autonomic reactivity in a first language than in a second language. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 561-579. Aycicegi, A., Gleason, J.B., & Harris, C.L. (2003). When is a first language more emotional? Psychophisiological evidence from bilingual speakers. In Press. Ayciegi, A., & Harris, C.L. (2003). Bilinguals' recall and recognition of emotion words. In Press. Balota, D.A., Larsen, R.J, & Mercer, K.A. (2006). Lexical characteristics of words used in Emotional Stroop Experiment. Emotion, Vol. 6, No.1, 62-72. Brysbaert, M., Duyck, W. What number translation studies can learn us about the lexicosemantic organization in bilinguals? In press. Blot, K.J., Paulus, P.B., & Zarate, M.A. (2003). Code-switching across brainstorming sessions: Implications for the revised hierarchical model of bilingual language processing. Vol. 50 (3), 171-183. Bradley, B.P., & Mogg, K. (2006). Time course of attentional bias for fear-relevant pictures in spider-fearful individuals Behaviour Research and Therapy, 44, 1241–1250. Bowman, H., Sharma, D., Wyble, B. Modeling the slow Emotional Stroop Effect. In press.
28
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Buchner, A., Mehl, B., & Rothermund, K. Artificially induced valence of distractor words increases the effects of irrelevant speech on serial recall. In press. Bunting, M.F., & Cowans, N. (2005). Working memory and flexibility in awareness and attention. Psychological research, Vol.69, No. 5-6, 412-419. Carlo, M.S., & Sylvester, E.S. (1996). Adult second-language reading research: How may it inform assessment and instruction. NCAL Technical Report TR96-08. Cherry, E. C. (1953) Some experiments on the recognition of speech, with one and with two ears. Journal of Acoustical Society of America, 25(5), pp. 975--979. Constantine, R., McNally, R. J., & Hornig, C.D. (2001). Snake fear and the pictorial emotional Stroop paradigm. Cognitive Therapy and Research, 25, 757–764. Condon, C.D., & Weinberger, N.M. (1991). Habituation produces frequency-specific plasticity of receptive fields in the auditory cortex. Behavioral Neuroscience, 105, 416-430. Corkin, S., & Kensinger, E.A. (2003). Memory enhancement for Emotional words: Are Emotional words more vividly remembered than neutral words? Memory and Cognition, 31 (8), 1169-1180. Corkin, S., & Kensinger, E.A. (2003). Effect of Negative Emotional content on Working memory and Long-Term memory. Emotion, Vol. 3, No.4, 378-393. Costa, A., Colome, A., & Caramzza, A. (2000). Lexical access in speech production: The bilingual case. Psicologica, 21, 403-437. Dehaene, S. (1999). Fitting two languages into one. Brain, Vol. 122, No.12, 2207-2208
29
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Dijkstra, T., Grainger, J., & van Heuven, W.J.B. (1999). Recognition of cognates and interlingual homographs: The neglected role of phonology. Journal of Memory and Language, 41, 496-518. Ekert-Centowska, A. The asymmetry in bilingual processing: Conceptual/lexical processing route and the word type effect. In press. Evans, P.D., Gilbert, S.L., Mekel-Bobrov, N., Vallender, E.J., Anderson, J.R., Vaez-Azizi, L.M., Tishkoff, S.A., Hudson, R.R., & Lahn, B.T. (2005). Microcephalin, a gene regulating brain size, continues to evolve adaptively in humans. Science, 309:1720. French, R.M., & Ohnesorge, C. (1995). Using non-cognate interlexical homographs to study Bilingual memory organization. In proceedings of the 17th Annual Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, pp.31-36. Finkbeiner, M., & Nicol, J. (2003). Semantic category effects in second language word learning. Applied Psycholinguistics, 24, 369-383. Federmeier, K.D., Kutas, M. (1998). Minding the body. Psychophysiology, 35, 135-150. Grosjean, F. (1998). Studying bilinguals: Methodological and conceptual issues. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 1, 131-149. Hariots, C., & Nelson, K. (2004). Bilingual memory: the interaction of Language and Thought. Bilingual Research Journal, 25: 4 Fall, 417-438. Hariots, C., & Nelson, K. (2004). Focusing on memory through a bilingual lens of understanding. Bilingual Research Journal 28:2 Summer, 181-205. Henik, A., Carter, C.S., Salo, R., Chaderjian, M., Kraft, L., Nordahl, T.E., & Robertson, L.C (2002). Attentional control and word inhibition in schizophrenia. Psychiatry Research, 110, 137-149.
30
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Kieras, D.E., Meyer, D.E., Mueller, S.T., & Seymour, T.L. (2003). Theoretical implications of articulatory duration, phonological similarity, and phonological complexity in verbal working memory. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory and Cognition, Vol. 29, No.6, 1353-1380. Kindt, M., & Brosschot, J. F. (1997). Phobia-related cognitive bias for pictorial and linguistic stimuli. Journal of Abnormal Psychology, 106, 644–648.
ARTICLE IN PRESS Lamers, M., & Roelofs, A. (2007). Modeling the control of visual attention in Stroop-like tasks. In press. Lavy, E., & van den Hout, M. (1993). Selective attention evidenced by pictorial and linguistic Stroop tasks. Behaviour Therapy, 24, 645–657. MacLeod, C.M. (1991). Half a century of Research on the Stroop Effect: an Integrative Review. Psychological Bulletin, Vol. 109, No. 2, 163-203. Marian, V., & Spivey, M. (2003). Competing activation in bilingual processing: within and between language competition. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition 6 (2), 97-115. McDonald, S.A., Tamariz, M., Thomson, J. (2004). Lexical level vs. conceptual level connections in bilingual lexicon: evidence from eye movements. In press. Monsell, S. (2003). Task Switching. Trends in Cognitive Science, Vol. 7, No.3, 134-140. Paivio, A. Dual-Coding theory and education. (2006). Draft chapter for the conference on "Pathway to Literacy Achievement for High Poverty Children". Perani, D., Paulesu, E., Galles, N.S., Dupoux, E., Dehaene, S., Bettinardi, V., Cappa, S.F., Fazio, F., & Mehler, J. (1998). The bilingual brain: proficiency and age of acquisition of the second language. Brain, 121, 1841-1852.
31
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Perez-Edgar K., Fox, N.A. (2003). Individual differences in children's performance during an Emotional Stroop task: a Behavioral and Electrophysiological study. Brain and Cognition, 52, 33-51. Platek, S.M., Thomson, J.W. (2007). Facial resemblance exaggerates sex-specific jealousybased decisions. Evolutionary Psychology, ISSN 1474-7049, Volume 5(1), pp. 223-231. Roelofs, A. (2003). Goal referenced selection of verbal action: Modeling attentional control in the Stroop task. Psychological Review, 110, No.1, 88-125. Roelofs, A. (2002). How do bilinguals control their use of languages? Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 5 (3), 214-215. Rommetveit, R. (2003). On the role of a "psychology of the second person studies of meaning, language and mind". Mind, Culture and Activity, 10 (3), 205-218. Salamoura, A., Williams, J.N. Backward word translation: Lexical vs. Conceptual mediation or "concept activation" vs. "word retrieval". In press. Smith, C.J., The role of individual differences in processing words with multiple translations. In press. Strayer, D.L, Drews, F.A., & Johnston, V.A. (2003). Cell phone induced failures of visual attention during simulated driving. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 9, 23-32. Tanenhaus, M.K., Flanigan, H.P., & Seidenberg, M.S. (1980). Orthographic and phonological activation in auditory and visual word recognition. Memoty and Cognition, 8, 513-420. Taura, H. (1998). Bilingual Dual-Coding in Japanese returnee students. Language, Culture and Curriculum, Vol.11, No.1, 47-70.
32
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Ventura. T., & Wolff, P. (2003). When Russians learn English: How the meaning of causal verbs may change. In proceedings of Twenty-Seventh Annual Boston University Conference on Language Development, 822-833. Williams, J.N. (2006). Incremental Interpretation in second language sentence processing. Bilingualism: Language and Cognition, 9 (1), 71-88. Watts, F., McKenna, F. P., Sharrock, R., & Trezise, L. (1986). Colour-naming of phobiarelated words. British Journal of Psychology, 77, 97–108. Watkins, M. J., & Peynircioglu, Z. F. (1983). On the nature of word recall: Evidence for linguistic specificity. Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 22, pp. 385–394.
33
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Appendixes Appendix I: Research group classification Control group M – Monolinguals (Israeli aborigines); test group BR – Bilinguals with Russian as a dominant language; test group BH –Bilinguals, Hebrew dominant). :אנא הגדיר\הגדירי את רמת שליטתך ברוסית.1 ) לא שולט כלל ) שולט ברמה בסיסית ) שולט ברמה בינונית ) שולט ברמה גבוהה ( אנא ציינו את הגיל בו עלית לארץ )אם אינך יליד הארץ.2 ?( במשך כמה שנים דיברת רוסית כשפה יחידה )אם רלוונטי לגביך.3
Appendix II: Test stimuli sample (1x1 scaled)
34
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Appendix III: Task words list classification to groups Stroop
English
Coherent
Coherent
Coherent
Word
Word
word
Transcript
word
word
word
Length in
Length in
[Hebrew]
ion
[Russian]
meaning
Type
syllables
syllables
[Hebrew]
[Russian]
פסלון
Pis-Lon
Slon
Elephant
Concrete
2
1
דגלון
Dig-Lon
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
פסגה
Pis-Ga
Pizda
Cunt
Taboo (3)
2
2
קרוב
Ka-Rov
Korova*
Cow
Concrete
2
3
מודאג
Mu-Dag
Mudak
Looser**
Taboo (2)
2
2
לבד
Le-Vad
Lebed
Swan
Concrete
2
2
דומה
Do-Me
Dooma***
Thought
Abstract
2
2
אבל
A-Val
Oval****
Oval
Concrete
2
2
דחוי
Da-Chooy
Hooy
Dick
Taboo (3)
2
1
סליל
Sleel
Slil
Peed
Taboo (1)
1
1
בלעדי
Bil-Adi
Bliadi
Whores
Taboo (3)
2
2
צל
Tzel
Tzel
Target
Abstract*
1
1
**** חרוש
Kha-Rush
Horosh***
Good
Abstract
2
2
Crow
Concrete
2
3
*** סרוק
Sa-Ruk
Soroka*** ****
בלבד
Bil-Vad
Blevat
To Puke
Taboo (1)
2
2
סוכה
Su-Ka
Suka
Bitch
Taboo (2)
2
2
רק
Rak
Rak
Cancer
Concrete*
1
1
2
2
******* ילדה
Yal-Da
Elda
Dick
Taboo (2) ******** *
דו"ח
Doo-Ach
Dooch
Spirit
Abstract
2
1
****זימה
Zee-Ma
Zima
Winter
Abstract
2
2
סם
Sum
Sam
Alone
Abstract
1
1
גרה
Ga-Ra
Gora*****
Mount
Concrete
2
2
******
*******
35
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
תפור
Ta-fur
Topor****
Axe
Concrete
2
2
********* אות
Ot
Ot
From
Abstract
1
1
נגע
Naga
Noga****
Foot
Concrete
2
2
********* * לוח
Lu-Ah
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
תורם
To-Rem
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
הולך
Ho-Leh
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
כרית
Ka-Rit
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
דייג
Da-Yag
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
אגס
A-Gas
None
None
Neutral
2
NA
קשה
Ka-Sha
Kasha
Porridge
Concrete
2
2
מורה
Mo-Ra
Mura
Nonsense
Concrete
2
2
*
Pronounced as "Ka-ro-va in Russian
**
Has an obscene meaning in Russian
****
Pronounced as "A-Val" in Russian
*****
Has two meanings in Russian – concrete and abstract (same as in English)
******
Pronounced as "Kha-Rosh" in Russian
*******
Pronounced as "Sa-Ro-Ka" in Russian
********
Has an additional meaning of a lethal illness (same as in English) and therefore might be seen as a threatening stimuli
*********
Rarely used in English
**********
Has a taboo meaning (Pornography related) in Hebrew
************
Pronounced as "Ga-Ra" in Russian
*************
Pronounced as "Ta-Por" in Russian
**************
Pronounced as "Na-Ga" in Russian
-
The blue silhouette around the number of syllables highlights equal word length of coherent words in Russian and Hebrew; in those cases the experiment results are more reliable.
36
The Open University of Israel Faculty of Social Sciences BA Psychology Program Ψ 2007–12–30
[email protected]
Appendix IV: ANOVA Summary Table
Source
SS
Main Effect A - Group Type Main Effect B - Stimuli Type Interaction A x B - Group*Stimuli Within Groups - Error Total
df
MS
F critical (α=0.05)
F
61394.17
2
30697.09
3.19
3.04
8518.72
3
2839.57
0.295
2.65
14064.17
6
2344.03
0.24
2.14
2193236.83
228
9619.46
2277213.9
240
Appendix V: Group Means Stimuli Type (Factor B)
Group Type (Factor A) Neutral
Concrete
Abstract
Taboo
Bilinguals L1 Hebrew
484.96
484.94
465.43
466.56
Bilinguals L1 Russian
482.93
489.4
485.4
484.83
Monolinguals (Hebrew Only)
461.98
440.26
459.73
429.18
Appendix VI: Tukey HSD Test Values Group Type/ Stimuli Type
Bilinguals Hebrew L1 Neutral
Concrete
Concrete
0.001
Abstract
0.89
0.89
Taboo
0.84
0.84
Bilinguals Russian L1 Abstract
Neutral
Concrete
Monolinguals Abstract
0.3 0.05
Neutral
Concrete
0.11
0.18
0.09
0.21
0.03
0.1
0.89
1.496
0.51
Appendix VII: Eta-Squared Data
Effect Group Type Stimuli Type Group - Stimuli Interaction Error
SS Effect 61394.17 8518.72 14064.17 2193236.83
SS Total 2277213.9 2277213.9 2277213.9 2277213.9
37
Abstract
0.99
Chi-Squared Value [%] 0.027 0.004 0.006 0.96
1.39